U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Display Settings:

Items per page

PMC Full-Text Search Results

Items: 7

1.
Fig. 1

Fig. 1. From: Dopamine reverses reward insensitivity in apathy following globus pallidus lesions.

Sections demonstrating the extent of basal ganglia lesions. KD's GPi lesion was larger on the left than on the right. The lesions are projected onto boundaries of the GPi (orange), GPe (yellow), putamen (green) and caudate (purple). The bottom left coronal section is a close up at the level of the anterior commissure.

Robert Adam, et al. Cortex. 2013 May;49(5):1292-1303.
2.
Fig. 2

Fig. 2. From: Dopamine reverses reward insensitivity in apathy following globus pallidus lesions.

Lesions and cortical connections. (A) For DTI analysis, three cortical sites are shown: LOFC (yellow), VMPFC (red) and M1 (blue). (B) Regression coefficients (betas) extracted from the voxel of maximum intensity within the lesion on the left (L) and right (R) for the three tracts. High values indicate that the tract passes through the lesion with a high probability.

Robert Adam, et al. Cortex. 2013 May;49(5):1292-1303.
3.
Fig. 4

Fig. 4. From: Dopamine reverses reward insensitivity in apathy following globus pallidus lesions.

Directional saccadic reward task. Participants attended a central fixation spot which was extinguished after 1000 msec of fixation. They then made a saccade as fast as possible to a target presented either to the left or right (50% each side). One side was rewarded while the other received no reward. The rewarded side (RS) remained constant for an unpredictable number of trials before switching to the other side.

Robert Adam, et al. Cortex. 2013 May;49(5):1292-1303.
4.
Fig. 6

Fig. 6. From: Dopamine reverses reward insensitivity in apathy following globus pallidus lesions.

Percentage early responses on traffic lights task (TLT) over time. (A) Over the course of the first session, healthy controls showed increased early responses but KD did not. (B) In the second session, an hour later, controls showed no further change but KD 1 h after receiving l-dopa showed escalating early responses. (C) During the drug holiday period (off l-dopa), KD's early responses reverted to pre-treatment levels.

Robert Adam, et al. Cortex. 2013 May;49(5):1292-1303.
5.
Fig. 3

Fig. 3. From: Dopamine reverses reward insensitivity in apathy following globus pallidus lesions.

Traffic lights task (TLT). (A) Subjects fixated a circle which successively turned red, amber and green. They were required not to move their eyes until the onset of the green light, otherwise they receive a small (constant) fine or punishment. To maximize reward, participants had to make a saccade to the contralateral target as quickly as possible after green light onset. (B) Amber durations were selected at random from a normal distribution (mean = 750 msec, SD = 125 msec). (C) Reward was calculated with a hyperbolically decaying function with a maximum value of 150 pence (£1.50) when SRT was zero. Thus to maximize reward subjects should program an eye movement to coincide with green light onset. However, amber durations were not constant and therefore they either had to take a risk (high reward or punishment) or wait for the green light before programming a saccade (low reward).

Robert Adam, et al. Cortex. 2013 May;49(5):1292-1303.
6.
Fig. 7

Fig. 7. From: Dopamine reverses reward insensitivity in apathy following globus pallidus lesions.

Results from the directional saccadic reward task. The control group (n = 12, arrows to side) showed a preference for the rewarded target locations, with significantly shorter SRTs. KD showed no reward preference at baseline, before treatment (Session 1). In Session 2 he was given a single dose (100 mg) of levodopa which led to a significant reward preference. This was maintained throughout chronic dopaminergic therapy (Sessions 3 Madopar 125 mg three times daily for 4 weeks, Session 4 Madopar CR 125 mg three times daily for 12 weeks). Following a treatment holiday (4 weeks), this reward preference was absent (Session 5). However, with subsequent treatment on the dopamine agonist ropinirole (1 mg three times a day), there was both a re-establishment of reward preference and significant decrease in latency to both rewarded and unrewarded targets. Error bars are +/− 1 SEM (standard error of the mean).

Robert Adam, et al. Cortex. 2013 May;49(5):1292-1303.
7.
Fig. 5

Fig. 5. From: Dopamine reverses reward insensitivity in apathy following globus pallidus lesions.

Traffic lights task (TLT): saccadic distributions. (A) Saccades for age-matched controls (n = 13) performing the TLT two distinct distributions: an early, anticipatory distribution and a later, reactive one made in response to green light onset. Early responses were divided into errors (saccades before the green light came on) and correct anticipations (saccades with < 200 msec latency after the green light). The plot here is for a total of 6500 saccades. (B) Pre-treatment, KD made mostly reactive saccades (461/500 trials [92.2%]) with a median latency of 248 msec. He made very few anticipatory saccades. (C) After treatment with l-DOPA 100 mg (Madopar CR 125 mg) three times a day for 12 weeks, there was a dramatic increase in early responding in KD. (D) After 12 weeks treatment with a dopamine agonist (ropinirole XL, 4 mg once a day), KD's distribution of saccades looks most similar to that of control subjects.

Robert Adam, et al. Cortex. 2013 May;49(5):1292-1303.

Display Settings:

Items per page

Supplemental Content

Recent activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...
Support Center