Format
Sort by

Send to

Choose Destination

Links from PubMed

Items: 1 to 20 of 164

1.

Dolphins maintain cognitive performance during 72 to 120 hours of continuous auditory vigilance.

Ridgway S, Keogh M, Carder D, Finneran J, Kamolnick T, Todd M, Goldblatt A.

J Exp Biol. 2009 May;212(Pt 10):1519-27. doi: 10.1242/jeb.027896.

2.

Dolphin continuous auditory vigilance for five days.

Ridgway S, Carder D, Finneran J, Keogh M, Kamolnick T, Todd M, Goldblatt A.

J Exp Biol. 2006 Sep;209(Pt 18):3621-8.

3.

Neural time and movement time in choice of whistle or pulse burst responses to different auditory stimuli by dolphins.

Ridgway SH.

J Acoust Soc Am. 2011 Feb;129(2):1073-80. doi: 10.1121/1.3523431.

PMID:
21361463
5.

Temporary threshold shift in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) exposed to mid-frequency tones.

Finneran JJ, Carder DA, Schlundt CE, Ridgway SH.

J Acoust Soc Am. 2005 Oct;118(4):2696-705.

PMID:
16266189
6.

Frequency and level dependent masking of the multiple auditory steady-state response in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus).

Branstetter BK, Finneran JJ, Houser DS.

J Acoust Soc Am. 2008 May;123(5):2928-35. doi: 10.1121/1.2896753.

PMID:
18529209
7.

Modified variance ratio for objective detection of transient evoked potentials in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus).

Finneran JJ.

J Acoust Soc Am. 2008 Dec;124(6):4069-82. doi: 10.1121/1.2996320.

PMID:
19206829
8.

Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) echoic angular discrimination: effects of object separation and complexity.

Branstetter BK, Mevissen SJ, Pack AA, Herman LM, Roberts SR, Carsrud LK.

J Acoust Soc Am. 2007 Jan;121(1):626-35.

PMID:
17297816
9.

Whistle rates of wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): influences of group size and behavior.

Quick NJ, Janik VM.

J Comp Psychol. 2008 Aug;122(3):305-11. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.122.3.305.

PMID:
18729659
10.
11.

[The recovery of the reactivity of the auditory system in the dolphin Tursiops truncatus to paired acoustic stimuli with different spectra].

Klishin VO, Popov VV, Supin AIa.

Zh Evol Biokhim Fiziol. 1991 May-Jun;27(3):314-9. Russian.

PMID:
1767609
12.
13.

Dolphins can maintain vigilant behavior through echolocation for 15 days without interruption or cognitive impairment.

Branstetter BK, Finneran JJ, Fletcher EA, Weisman BC, Ridgway SH.

PLoS One. 2012;7(10):e47478. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047478.

14.

Vocal reporting of echolocation targets: dolphins often report before click trains end.

Ridgway SH, Elsberry WR, Blackwood DJ, Kamolnick T, Todd M, Carder DA, Chaplin M, Cranford TW.

J Acoust Soc Am. 2012 Jan;131(1):593-8. doi: 10.1121/1.3664074.

PMID:
22280621
15.

Beamwidth control and angular target detection in an echolocating bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus).

Moore PW, Dankiewicz LA, Houser DS.

J Acoust Soc Am. 2008 Nov;124(5):3324-32. doi: 10.1121/1.2980453. Erratum in: J Acoust Soc Am. 2009 Aug;126(2):920.

PMID:
19045815
16.

Whistle emissions of Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) differ with group composition and surface behaviors.

Hawkins ER, Gartside DF.

J Acoust Soc Am. 2010 Apr;127(4):2652-63. doi: 10.1121/1.3308465.

PMID:
20370046
17.

Bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) moan as low in frequency as baleen whales.

van der Woude SE.

J Acoust Soc Am. 2009 Sep;126(3):1552. doi: 10.1121/1.3177272.

PMID:
19739768
18.

Estimated communication range of social sounds used by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus).

Quintana-Rizzo E, Mann DA, Wells RS.

J Acoust Soc Am. 2006 Sep;120(3):1671-83.

PMID:
17004488
19.

Laterality of flipper rubbing behaviour in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus): caused by asymmetry of eye use?

Sakai M, Hishii T, Takeda S, Kohshima S.

Behav Brain Res. 2006 Jun 30;170(2):204-10.

PMID:
16569444
20.
Items per page

Supplemental Content

Support Center