Format
Sort by
Items per page

Send to

Choose Destination

Search results

Items: 1 to 50 of 94

1.

Suboptimal choice and initial-link requirement.

Pisklak JM, McDevitt MA, Dunn RM, Spetch ML.

J Exp Anal Behav. 2019 Nov;112(3):242-253. doi: 10.1002/jeab.553. Epub 2019 Nov 4.

PMID:
31680265
2.

The Near-Miss Effect in Slot Machines: A Review and Experimental Analysis Over Half a Century Later.

Pisklak JM, Yong JJH, Spetch ML.

J Gambl Stud. 2019 Sep 14. doi: 10.1007/s10899-019-09891-8. [Epub ahead of print]

PMID:
31522339
3.

Same but different: Socially foraging ants backtrack like individually foraging ants but use different mechanisms.

Freas CA, Congdon JV, Plowes NJR, Spetch ML.

J Insect Physiol. 2019 Oct;118:103944. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2019.103944. Epub 2019 Sep 11.

PMID:
31520596
4.

Not just going with the flow: foraging ants attend to polarised light even while on the pheromone trail.

Freas CA, Plowes NJR, Spetch ML.

J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 2019 Oct;205(5):755-767. doi: 10.1007/s00359-019-01363-z. Epub 2019 Aug 17.

PMID:
31422422
5.

The power of nothing: Risk preference in pigeons, but not people, is driven primarily by avoidance of zero outcomes.

Pisklak JM, Madan CR, Ludvig EA, Spetch ML.

J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2019 Oct;45(4):431-445. doi: 10.1037/xan0000217. Epub 2019 Aug 15.

PMID:
31414880
6.

Terrestrial cue learning and retention during the outbound and inbound foraging trip in the desert ant, Cataglyphis velox.

Freas CA, Spetch ML.

J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 2019 Apr;205(2):177-189. doi: 10.1007/s00359-019-01316-6. Epub 2019 Jan 28.

PMID:
30689018
7.

Comparative inspiration: From puzzles with pigeons to novel discoveries with humans in risky choice.

Madan CR, Ludvig EA, Spetch ML.

Behav Processes. 2019 Mar;160:10-19. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.12.009. Epub 2019 Jan 3. Review.

8.

Anticipation of a midsession reversal in humans.

McMillan N, Spetch ML.

Behav Processes. 2019 Feb;159:60-64. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2018.12.016. Epub 2019 Jan 3.

PMID:
30611851
9.

Frequency and value both matter in the suboptimal choice procedure.

Pisklak JM, McDevitt MA, Dunn RM, Spetch ML.

J Exp Anal Behav. 2019 Jan;111(1):1-11. doi: 10.1002/jeab.490. Epub 2018 Dec 19.

PMID:
30569554
10.

Odometry and backtracking: social and individual navigation in group foraging desert harvester ants (Veromessor pergandei).

Plowes N, Du Y, Congdon JV, Bulitko V, Soares ES, Spetch ML.

Anim Cogn. 2019 Jan;22(1):35-47. doi: 10.1007/s10071-018-1218-7. Epub 2018 Oct 17.

PMID:
30334125
11.

Living near the edge: How extreme outcomes and their neighbors drive risky choice.

Ludvig EA, Madan CR, McMillan N, Xu Y, Spetch ML.

J Exp Psychol Gen. 2018 Dec;147(12):1905-1918. doi: 10.1037/xge0000414. Epub 2018 Mar 22.

PMID:
29565605
12.

The contribution of nonrigid motion and shape information to object perception in pigeons and humans.

Nankoo JF, Madan CR, Sawalha J, Wylie DR, Friedman A, Spetch ML, Vuong QC.

J Vis. 2017 Jun 1;17(6):17. doi: 10.1167/17.6.17.

PMID:
28654962
13.

Cue integration in spatial search for jointly learned landmarks but not for separately learned landmarks.

Du Y, McMillan N, Madan CR, Spetch ML, Mou W.

J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2017 Dec;43(12):1857-1871. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000416. Epub 2017 May 15.

PMID:
28504533
14.

Chickadees discriminate contingency reversals presented consistently, but not frequently.

McMillan N, Hahn AH, Congdon JV, Campbell KA, Hoang J, Scully EN, Spetch ML, Sturdy CB.

Anim Cogn. 2017 Jul;20(4):655-663. doi: 10.1007/s10071-017-1088-4. Epub 2017 Apr 5.

PMID:
28382429
15.

Look up: Human adults use vertical height cues in reorientation.

Du Y, Spetch ML, Mou W.

Mem Cognit. 2016 Nov;44(8):1277-1287.

PMID:
27315804
16.

Sensitivity of the avian motion system to light and dark stimuli.

Nankoo JF, Madan CR, Spetch ML, Wylie DR.

Exp Brain Res. 2017 Feb;235(2):401-406. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4786-2. Epub 2016 Oct 14.

PMID:
27743012
17.

The role of memory in distinguishing risky decisions from experience and description.

Madan CR, Ludvig EA, Spetch ML.

Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2017 Oct;70(10):2048-2059. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1220608. Epub 2016 Sep 7.

PMID:
27602887
18.

Fast- and slow-exploring pigeons differ in how they use previously learned rules.

Guillette LM, Baron DM, Sturdy CB, Spetch ML.

Behav Processes. 2017 Jan;134:54-62. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.07.006. Epub 2016 Aug 25. Review.

PMID:
27567525
19.

Cue salience influences the use of height cues in reorientation in pigeons (Columba livia).

Du Y, Mahdi N, Paul B, Spetch ML.

J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2016 Jul;42(3):273-80. doi: 10.1037/xan0000106.

PMID:
27379717
20.

Multiple cue use and integration in pigeons (Columba livia).

Legge EL, Madan CR, Spetch ML, Ludvig EA.

Anim Cogn. 2016 May;19(3):581-91. doi: 10.1007/s10071-016-0963-8. Epub 2016 Feb 23.

PMID:
26908004
21.

Pigeons perform poorly on a midsession reversal task without rigid temporal regularity.

McMillan N, Sturdy CB, Pisklak JM, Spetch ML.

Anim Cogn. 2016 Jul;19(4):855-9. doi: 10.1007/s10071-016-0962-9. Epub 2016 Feb 17.

PMID:
26886221
22.

When good news leads to bad choices.

McDevitt MA, Dunn RM, Spetch ML, Ludvig EA.

J Exp Anal Behav. 2016 Jan;105(1):23-40. doi: 10.1002/jeab.192. Review.

PMID:
26781050
23.

When good pigeons make bad decisions: Choice with probabilistic delays and outcomes.

Pisklak JM, McDevitt MA, Dunn RM, Spetch ML.

J Exp Anal Behav. 2015 Nov;104(3):241-51. doi: 10.1002/jeab.177.

PMID:
26676182
24.

Avian cognition: examples of sophisticated capabilities in space and song.

McMillan N, Hahn AH, Spetch ML, Sturdy CB.

Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci. 2015 May-Jun;6(3):285-97. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1346. Epub 2015 Mar 16. Review.

PMID:
26263230
25.

When is a choice not a choice? Pigeons fail to inhibit incorrect responses on a go/no-go midsession reversal task.

McMillan N, Sturdy CB, Spetch ML.

J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn. 2015 Jul;41(3):255-65. doi: 10.1037/xan0000058. Epub 2015 Apr 27.

PMID:
25915749
26.

Re-evaluating birds' ability to detect Glass patterns.

Nankoo JF, Madan CR, Wylie DR, Spetch ML.

Anim Cogn. 2015 Jul;18(4):945-52. doi: 10.1007/s10071-015-0865-1. Epub 2015 Apr 1.

PMID:
25823884
27.

Visualizing and quantifying movement from pre-recorded videos: The spectral time-lapse (STL) algorithm.

Madan CR, Spetch ML.

F1000Res. 2014 Jan 21;3:19. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.3-19.v1. eCollection 2014.

28.

Priming memories of past wins induces risk seeking.

Ludvig EA, Madan CR, Spetch ML.

J Exp Psychol Gen. 2015 Feb;144(1):24-9. doi: 10.1037/xge0000046. Epub 2014 Dec 22.

PMID:
25528669
29.

Temporal summation of global form signals in dynamic Glass patterns.

Nankoo JF, Madan CR, Spetch ML, Wylie DR.

Vision Res. 2015 Feb;107:30-5. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.10.033. Epub 2014 Nov 20.

30.

Combining sky and earth: desert ants (Melophorus bagoti) show weighted integration of celestial and terrestrial cues.

Legge EL, Wystrach A, Spetch ML, Cheng K.

J Exp Biol. 2014 Dec 1;217(Pt 23):4159-66. doi: 10.1242/jeb.107862. Epub 2014 Oct 16.

31.

Reward context determines risky choice in pigeons and humans.

Ludvig EA, Madan CR, Pisklak JM, Spetch ML.

Biol Lett. 2014 Aug;10(8). pii: 20140451. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0451.

32.

Practice makes proficient: pigeons (Columba livia) learn efficient routes on full-circuit navigational traveling salesperson problems.

Baron DM, Ramirez AJ, Bulitko V, Madan CR, Greiner A, Hurd PL, Spetch ML.

Anim Cogn. 2015 Jan;18(1):53-64. doi: 10.1007/s10071-014-0776-6. Epub 2014 Jun 26.

PMID:
24966123
33.

Perception of complex motion in humans and pigeons (Columba livia).

Nankoo JF, Madan CR, Spetch ML, Wylie DR.

Exp Brain Res. 2014 Jun;232(6):1843-53. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3876-2. Epub 2014 Feb 26.

PMID:
24570385
34.

Use of geometric properties of landmark arrays for reorientation relative to remote cities and local objects.

Mou W, Nankoo JF, Zhou R, Spetch ML.

J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2014 Mar;40(2):476-91. doi: 10.1037/a0034976. Epub 2013 Nov 18.

PMID:
24245534
35.

Remembering the best and worst of times: memories for extreme outcomes bias risky decisions.

Madan CR, Ludvig EA, Spetch ML.

Psychon Bull Rev. 2014 Jun;21(3):629-36. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0542-9.

PMID:
24189991
36.

Reorientation in diamond-shaped environments: encoding of features and angles in enclosures versus arrays by adult humans and pigeons (Columbia livia).

Lubyk DM, Spetch ML, Zhou R, Pisklak J, Mou W.

Anim Cogn. 2013 Jul;16(4):565-81. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0594-7. Epub 2013 Jan 9.

PMID:
23299225
37.

Object location memory: integration and competition between multiple context objects but not between observers' body and context objects.

Mou W, Spetch ML.

Cognition. 2013 Feb;126(2):181-97. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.09.018. Epub 2012 Nov 9.

PMID:
23142038
38.

Perception of dynamic glass patterns.

Nankoo JF, Madan CR, Spetch ML, Wylie DR.

Vision Res. 2012 Nov 1;72:55-62. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.09.008. Epub 2012 Sep 24.

39.

Not all locations are created equal: exploring how adults hide and search for objects.

Legge EL, Spetch ML, Cenkner A, Bulitko V, Anderson C, Brown M, Heth D.

PLoS One. 2012;7(5):e36993. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036993. Epub 2012 May 11.

40.

Geometric orientation by humans: angles weigh in.

Lubyk DM, Dupuis B, GutiƩrrez L, Spetch ML.

Psychon Bull Rev. 2012 Jun;19(3):436-42. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0232-z.

PMID:
22382695
41.

Is the enhancement of memory due to reward driven by value or salience?

Madan CR, Spetch ML.

Acta Psychol (Amst). 2012 Feb;139(2):343-9. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.12.010. Epub 2012 Jan 20.

PMID:
22266252
42.

Finding the best angle: pigeons (Columba livia) weight angular information more heavily than relative wall length in an open-field geometry task.

Lubyk DM, Spetch ML.

Anim Cogn. 2012 May;15(3):305-12. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0454-x. Epub 2011 Sep 15.

PMID:
21918871
43.

Of black swans and tossed coins: is the description-experience gap in risky choice limited to rare events?

Ludvig EA, Spetch ML.

PLoS One. 2011;6(6):e20262. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020262. Epub 2011 Jun 1.

44.

Strategies in landmark use by orangutans and human children.

Marsh HL, Spetch ML, MacDonald SE.

Anim Cogn. 2011 Jul;14(4):487-502. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0382-9. Epub 2011 Feb 11.

PMID:
21312049
45.

Reward magnitude and timing in pigeons.

Ludvig EA, Balci F, Spetch ML.

Behav Processes. 2011 Mar;86(3):359-63. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2011.01.003. Epub 2011 Jan 16.

PMID:
21244854
46.

Proximity to an edge affects search strategy in adults and children.

Batty ER, Spetch ML, Parent M.

Behav Processes. 2010 Oct;85(3):265-77. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2010.07.015. Epub 2010 Aug 11.

PMID:
20708662
47.

Not using the obvious: desert ants, Melophorus bagoti, learn local vectors but not beacons in an arena.

Legge EL, Spetch ML, Cheng K.

Anim Cogn. 2010 Nov;13(6):849-60. doi: 10.1007/s10071-010-0333-x. Epub 2010 Jun 22.

PMID:
20567867
48.

Contributions of category and fine-grained information to location memory: when categories don't weigh in.

Spetch ML, Friedman A, Bialowas J, Verbeek E.

Mem Cognit. 2010 Mar;38(2):154-62. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.2.154.

PMID:
20173188
49.

Categories and range effects in human spatial memory.

Cheng K, Spetch ML, Hoan A.

Front Psychol. 2010 Dec 21;1:231. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00231. eCollection 2010.

50.

Using perceptrons to explore the reorientation task.

Dawson MR, Kelly DM, Spetch ML, Dupuis B.

Cognition. 2010 Feb;114(2):207-26. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.006. Epub 2009 Sep 30.

PMID:
19796761

Supplemental Content

Loading ...
Support Center