Format
Sort by
Items per page

Send to

Choose Destination

Links from PubMed

Items: 1 to 20 of 92

1.

Voluntary action expands perceived duration of its sensory consequence.

Park J, Schlag-Rey M, Schlag J.

Exp Brain Res. 2003 Apr;149(4):527-9. Epub 2003 Feb 26.

PMID:
12677334
2.

Illusory perceptions of space and time preserve cross-saccadic perceptual continuity.

Yarrow K, Haggard P, Heal R, Brown P, Rothwell JC.

Nature. 2001 Nov 15;414(6861):302-5.

PMID:
11713528
3.

Spatio-temporal contingency of saccade-induced chronostasis.

Georg K, Lappe M.

Exp Brain Res. 2007 Jul;180(3):535-9. Epub 2007 Feb 8.

PMID:
17287990
4.

Consistent chronostasis effects across saccade categories imply a subcortical efferent trigger.

Yarrow K, Johnson H, Haggard P, Rothwell JC.

J Cogn Neurosci. 2004 Jun;16(5):839-47.

5.

Spatio-temporal topography of saccadic overestimation of time.

Knöll J, Morrone MC, Bremmer F.

Vision Res. 2013 May 3;83:56-65. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2013.02.013. Epub 2013 Feb 28.

6.

A metanalysis of the effect of the Müller-Lyer illusion on saccadic eye movements: no general support for a dissociation of perception and oculomotor action.

Bruno N, Knox PC, de Grave DD.

Vision Res. 2010 Dec;50(24):2671-82. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.016. Epub 2010 Sep 19. Review.

7.

Why are saccades influenced by the Brentano illusion?

de Grave DD, Smeets JB, Brenner E.

Exp Brain Res. 2006 Oct;175(1):177-82. Epub 2006 May 30.

PMID:
16733700
8.

Saccades to remembered targets: the effects of smooth pursuit and illusory stimulus motion.

Zivotofsky AZ, Rottach KG, Averbuch-Heller L, Kori AA, Thomas CW, Dell'Osso LF, Leigh RJ.

J Neurophysiol. 1996 Dec;76(6):3617-32.

PMID:
8985862
9.

The influence of the Brentano illusion on eye and hand movements.

de Grave DD, Franz VH, Gegenfurtner KR.

J Vis. 2006 Jun 29;6(7):727-38.

PMID:
16895455
10.

Chronostasis without voluntary action.

Alexander I, Thilo KV, Cowey A, Walsh V.

Exp Brain Res. 2005 Feb;161(1):125-32. Epub 2004 Dec 7.

PMID:
15586278
11.

Perception of temporal duration affected by automatic and controlled movements.

Herai T, Mogi K.

Conscious Cogn. 2014 Oct;29:23-35. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.07.012. Epub 2014 Aug 8.

12.

Illusory shifts in visual direction accompany adaptation of saccadic eye movements.

Bahcall DO, Kowler E.

Nature. 1999 Aug 26;400(6747):864-6.

PMID:
10476963
13.

The type of visual information mediates eye and hand movement bias when aiming to a Müller-Lyer illusion.

Lavrysen A, Helsen WF, Elliott D, Buekers MJ, Feys P, Heremans E.

Exp Brain Res. 2006 Oct;174(3):544-54. Epub 2006 May 23.

PMID:
16645876
14.
15.

Spatial consequences of bridging the saccadic gap.

Yarrow K, Whiteley L, Rothwell JC, Haggard P.

Vision Res. 2006 Feb;46(4):545-55. Epub 2005 Jul 7.

16.
17.

Sensory processing of motor inaccuracy depends on previously performed movement and on subsequent motor corrections: a study of the saccadic system.

Panouillères M, Urquizar C, Salemme R, Pélisson D.

PLoS One. 2011 Feb 23;6(2):e17329. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017329.

18.

Microsaccades drive illusory motion in the Enigma illusion.

Troncoso XG, Macknik SL, Otero-Millan J, Martinez-Conde S.

Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008 Oct 14;105(41):16033-8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0709389105. Epub 2008 Oct 8.

19.

Action, arousal, and subjective time.

Yarrow K, Haggard P, Rothwell JC.

Conscious Cogn. 2004 Jun;13(2):373-90.

PMID:
15134766
20.

The effects of illusory line motion on incongruent saccades: implications for saccadic eye movements and visual attention.

Crawford TJ, Kean M, Klein RM, Hamm JP.

Exp Brain Res. 2006 Aug;173(3):498-506. Epub 2006 Mar 10.

PMID:
16528498

Supplemental Content

Support Center