• We are sorry, but NCBI web applications do not support your browser and may not function properly. More information
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC Dec 6, 2012.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC3516401
NIHMSID: NIHMS383637

Retronasal odor representations in the dorsal olfactory bulb of rats

Abstract

Animals perceive their olfactory environment not only from odors originating in the external world (orthonasal route) but also from odors released in the oral cavity while eating food (retronasal route). Retronasal olfaction is crucial for the perception of food flavor in humans. However, little is known about the retronasal stimulus coding in the brain. The most basic question is if and how route affects the odor representations at the level of the olfactory bulb (OB), where odor quality codes originate. We used optical calcium imaging of presynaptic dorsal OB responses to odorants in anesthetized rats to ask whether the rat OB could be activated retronasally, and how these responses compare to orthonasal responses under similar conditions. We further investigated the effects of specific odorant properties on orthoversus retronasal response patterns. We found that at a physiologically relevant flow rate retronasal odorants can effectively reach the olfactory receptor neurons, eliciting glomerular response patterns that grossly overlap with those of orthonasal responses, but differ from the orthonasal patterns in the response amplitude and temporal dynamics. Interestingly, such differences correlated well with specific odorant properties. Less volatile odorants yielded relatively smaller responses retronasally, but volatility did not affect relative temporal profiles. More polar odorants responded with relatively longer onset latency and time to peak retronasally, but polarity did not affect relative response magnitudes. These data provide insight into the early stages of retronasal stimulus coding and establish relationships between ortho- and retronasal odor representations in the rat OB.

Keywords: orthonasal, retronasal, odor maps, olfactory bulb, glomerular response patterns, calcium imaging, polarity, volatility, flavor

Introduction

In mammals odorants can reach the olfactory receptor neurons (ORNs) by two routes: orthonasally, when volatiles enter the nasal cavity during inhalation/sniffing, and retronasally, when food volatiles released in the mouth pass into the nasal cavity during exhalation/eating. Rozin (Rozin, 1982) considered these routes as two distinct modes of olfaction and hypothesized that the perception of odorants depends on odor route. Several psychophysical (Pierce and Halpern, 1996; Heilmann and Hummel, 2004; Sun and Halpern, 2005) and human brain imaging studies (Small et al., 2005) support this hypothesis, and suggest that ortho- and retronasal delivery of the same odorant evokes distinct perceptions and patterns of neural response in the cortical areas of the brain. However, mechanisms underlying these differences remain unknown. The most fundamental question is if and how ortho- and retronasal odor representations differ from each other at the level of the olfactory bulb (OB), the first synaptic relay in the olfactory system, where odor quality codes originate (Ressler et al., 1993; Vassar et al., 1993; Mori et al., 1999). In this study we addressed this basic question by calcium imaging of presynaptic glomerular terminals from olfactory receptor neurons using a rat model.

Odorant coding primarily involves the transformation of an odorant’s molecular features into a rough spatial map of activated glomeruli in the OB. Additionally, the temporal dynamics of the glomerular responses may also contribute to the odor quality coding at the level of the OB (Spors et al., 2006; Wesson et al., 2008; Carey et al., 2009; Junek et al., 2010). Several optical imaging studies of the spatiotemporal glomerular activity reflecting presynaptic responses (Wachowiak and Cohen, 2001; Verhagen et al., 2007; Wesson et al., 2008) and time-integrated combined presynaptic and postsynaptic responses (Xu et al., 2003; Johnson and Leon, 2007) have revealed orthonasal odor coding mechanisms at the OB. However, glomerular responses to the retronasal stimulation remain unexplored. In this study, using optical calcium imaging of presynaptic dorsal OB responses to a variety of odorants, we tested the hypothesis that distinct spatiotemporal glomerular activity patterns exist for ortho- versus retronasal routes of the same odorant. We speculate that these differences may contribute to the distinct cortical responses observed in humans.

Our hypothesis is based on reports supporting a ‘chromatographic’ model of olfactory epithelium, which suggests that odor flow parameters (Mozell et al., 1984) and odorant distribution across the olfactory epithelium (Scott-Johnson et al., 2000) can influence ORN responses. Indeed, route seems to influence the air-flow pattern inside the nasal cavity (Zhao et al., 2004; Zhao et al., 2006), as well as the olfactory population response to odorants at the level of the ORNs in vivo in rats (Scott et al., 2007). Here we show that route also influences the spatio-temporal glomerular activity patterns at the OB, and that this effect depends on specific odorant properties, such as polarity and volatility. Since retronasal smell is an essential element of flavor, the present study expands our understanding of the neural bases of flavor perception.

Methods

Subjects

Long-Evans female rats weighing 180–200 g were purchased from Charles River Laboratories Inc. (New York, USA) and housed individually in an environment of controlled humidity (60%) and temperature (23°C). The vivarium was set with 12-h light-dark cycles and all the experiments were carried out in the light phase. All the animals were treated according to the guidelines established by the U.S. National Institutes of Health (1986), and the experimental protocols were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the John B. Pierce Laboratory. Data acquired from nine rats are presented here.

Labeling olfactory receptor neurons

Olfactory receptor neurons in the dorsal recess of the nasal cavity were loaded bilaterally with dextran-conjugated calcium-sensitive dye (Oregon Green BAPTA 488-1 dextran; Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA) using a well-established protocol (Wachowiak and Cohen, 2001), adapted for rats (Verhagen et al., 2007). Animals were held 8–12 days before recording.

Optical window and double tracheotomy surgery

Prior to imaging, the dye-infused rats were anesthetized with urethane (1.5 g/kg ip), the bone overlying the dorsal surface of the bulb was exposed, thinned and coated with cyanoacrylate glue to make the bone transparent (Bozza et al., 2004). A double tracheotomy surgery was performed allowing for the rat to sniff artificially. A Teflon tube (OD 2.1 mm, upper tracheotomy tube) was inserted 10 mm into the nasopharynx, to assure that airflow was restricted to the nose (the epiglottis could otherwise leak air flow via the oral cavity). Another Teflon tube (OD 2.3 mm, lower tracheotomy tube) was inserted into the caudal end of the tracheal cut. Both tubes were fixed and sealed to the tissues using surgical thread and cyanoacrylate glue. The head was stabilized by gluing it to a bar mounted on a stereotaxic head holder designed not to interfere with tracheal breathing. A custom made Teflon nose-mask with minimal dead-space was applied to the clean-shaved nose for orthonasal odor delivery. The upper tracheotomy tube inserted into the nasopharynx was used to deliver odors retronasally (Fig. 1). Local anesthetic (2% Lidocaine) was applied at all pressure points and incisions. Artificial sniffing was synchronized to the start of each trial. Throughout the surgery and optical recordings rats’ core body temperature was maintained at 37°C with a thermostatically controlled heating pad (Omega Engineering Inc, Stamford, CT).

Figure 1
Calcium imaging of the olfactory bulb (OB) glomerular responses to ortho- and retronasal odorant stimulations. The experimental setup is shown in A and B. A, schematic drawing of the double-tracheotomized rat showing tightly sealed orthonasal nose mask ...

Optical recordings

Optical calcium signals from the dorsal OB were recorded using a CCD camera (Redshirt Imaging LLC, Decatur, GA, USA) with 256 × 256 pixel resolution, and at a frame rate of 25 Hz. This resolution was sufficient to identify single glomeruli at magnifications low enough to image across the entire dorsal and lateral surface of the bulb. The epifluorescence macroscope used was a custom made tandem-lens type (Ratzlaff and Grinvald, 1991) with ~2× magnification and high NA (0.85–0.95) CCTV objectives for high SNR. A high power LED (Luxeon LXHL-PE09, Philips Lumileds, San Jose, CA, USA) driven by a linear DC power supply acted as the light source. A custom made DC amplifier (based on a linear Apex power operational amplifier; Cirrus Logic, Inc., Austin, TX, USA) powered a peltier (Melcor, OT2.0-31-F1) device onto which the LED was glued. The LED-cooling peltier current was proportional to the LED current, yielding a stable illumination. The fluorescence filter set used was FF01-475/50-50 (excitation filter), LP515 (dichroic), and LP530 (emission filter; Semrock, Lake Forest, IL, USA). This system provided fast imaging capabilities, a large field of view, and low noise. Raw images were converted to images representing the relative change in fluorescence (%ΔF/F) in each pixel and frame after stimulus application. Data analysis was performed using NeuroPlex software (RedShirtImaging LLC, GA, USA), and routines were written in Matlab (Version 7.11.0, The MathWorks Inc., Natick MA, USA).

Ortho- and retronasal odorant presentation and odorants

The schematics of the experimental setup and examples of ortho- and retronasal imaging trials are shown in Fig. 1. We employed a novel bi-directional artificial sniffing paradigm with tightly sealed orthonasal nose mask and retronasal odor tube in double-tracheotomized rats (Fig. 1 A & B). This closed bi-directional ‘positive-pressure artificial sniffing paradigm’, also connected to a pressure sensor (Honeywell, Morristown, NJ, USA; part 24PCAFA6G) to measure the flow-resistance along both the routes, enabled us to properly control the delivery of the odorant stimuli ortho- as well as retronasally. The time to fill the dead volume was approximately 120 ms for either route. The orthonasal stimulus delivery occurred ~2.4 ms earlier than retronasal delivery (based on 0.01 ml difference in dead-volume and a flow rate time of 240 ms/ml). We chose 250 ml/min as this was found by Youngentob et al. to be the average flow rate of in- and expiratory sniffing by awake behaving rats (see Table 2 there, 1.9–8.9 ml/s) (Youngentob et al., 1987). The nose-mask guided the air out of the nasal cavity to the exhaust vent during retronasal stimulation, and guided the air into the nasal cavity from the olfactometer during orthonasal stimulation. The Teflon valves (NResearch Inc., NJ, USA) involved in this paradigm were fully automated by a program written in LabVIEW (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA). The stimulation paradigm for both the routes was identical and consisted of two 2s artificial sniffs of an odorant at an interval of 3s. All the results are based on the responses to the first odor pulse only.

Table 2
Summary of multivariate analysis of variance showing factors affecting glomerular response pattern and interaction between factors.

Each imaging session consisted of 80 to 140 trials triggered manually with an iti of >3 min. The order of stimulus presentation was randomly permuted for each route and odorant. In each trial odorants were presented at 250 ml/min flow-rate as two 2s pulses separated by 3s interval (Fig. 1 C–D) using a custom-built multichannel auto-switching flow dilution olfactometer (Lam et al., 2000) with dedicated lines for each odorant to avoid cross-contamination. This allowed for the continuous control of odorant concentration over 1.5 log units. After each stimulus the nasal cavity was flushed with clean humidified (sparging distilled water) air for one minute. The olfactometer output was routed to a set of route-switching valves that were mounted on the side of the stereotax so as to minimize the dead space. Odorant concentrations are indicated as percentage saturated vapor (% s.v.). Medical-grade air was used to dilute the vapor in the headspace of odorant reservoirs to generate the desired concentration. Odorants were diluted prior to reaching common tubing to maximize purity. The odor manifolds were automatically flushed with clean air after each stimulation cycle. Our system allowed precise control of the odorant access to the nasal cavity both ortho- and retronasally. With this paradigm we were able to control intranasal odorant concentration as well as air flow-rates. Monomolecular odorants were chosen from the family of odorants whose effects on the dorsal bulb have been previously characterized (Johnson and Leon, 2000; Uchida et al., 2000; Meister and Bonhoeffer, 2001; Wachowiak and Cohen, 2001). In collaboration with Dr. Johnson and Leon we identified sixteen odorants that are most selective for the dorsal bulb, based on quantitative MATLAB analyses of their raw 2-DG datasets. These odorants with their partition coefficients and other parameters (EPI estimates) are shown in Table 1. The entire odorant delivery system was made of Teflon. All the odorants were obtained from Sigma-Aldrich (St. Louis, MO, USA) and stored under nitrogen in the dark.

Table 1
Odorants with some of their physicochemical properties. These odorants are known to activate dorsal OB in rats.

Measurement of breathing

Breathing was measured as the movement of the thorax by a piezoelectric strap around the animal’s chest. During each respiration cycle, one sharp upward deflection in the piezoelectric signal occurred during thorax expansion (inspiration). The point of onset of this deflection occurring before and after the stimulus onset time was used as a time reference for estimating instantaneous breathing frequency and assessing occurrence of response coupling with breathing cycle. The temporal parameters were measured in reference to the stimulus onset time recorded directly by a pressure sensor connected to the bidirectional artificial sniffing setup.

Data analysis

Identification of activated glomeruli

Datasets consisting of optical images of 256 × 256 pixels sampled at 25Hz, pressure signals sampled at 200Hz, breathing signals, information on odor identity, odor concentration and flow-rate were acquired using Neuroplex software on a 12- sec trial-by-trial basis (Fig. 1 C–D). Script files written in MATLAB were used to extract and ‘preprocess’ these data to correct global noise in every imaged frame. The preprocessed images were then averaged across trials for each stimulus to identify regions of interest (ROI, activated glomeruli). Focal changes in fluorescence in the OB have been shown previously to correspond to individual glomeruli (Belluscio and Katz, 2001; Meister and Bonhoeffer, 2001; Wachowiak et al., 2004).

Estimation of response magnitude

Using the identified ROI we then extracted average glomerular response curves (F-traces) based on stimulus onset times. These F-traces guided the selection of optimum pre-frame (before stimulus onset) and post-frame (about max response) windows which consisted of 15–21 frames (600–840 ms). The response magnitudes across each ROI were then measured using this window for each trial as percent change in fluorescence before and after stimulus onset (% ΔF/F) as reported previously (Verhagen et al., 2007). MANOVA (odorant × route) was then performed across all trials from an animal, and any ROI for which effect of odorant (including odorless air, delivered via a separate clean line) was not significant was removed from further analyses.

Spatial analysis of ortho- vs. retronasal response patterns

Peak response amplitudes (% ΔF/F) at the ROIs were compared between routes of stimulation across odorants. Correlation analysis and ANOVA were used to establish whether changing the route of stimulation changes the spatial odor-map, and how the ortho- vs. retronasal differences were affected by physicochemical properties of the odorants. Multidimensional scaling was performed in Systat (v10.2, method: Kruskal, mono). Averages are reported ± sem (s.d. / √n). Alpha-level was set at 0.05.

Temporal analysis of ortho- vs. retronasal response pattern

To measure temporal parameters of the glomerular response a custom algorithm was developed that fitted the optical signals from each ROI to a double sigmoid function as described previously (Wesson et al., 2008; Carey et al., 2009). The analysis allowed robust and objective measurement of response timing. Briefly, the signal from each ROI was band-pass filtered (second-order Butterworth, 0.4–8 Hz) followed by fourth-order Daubechies wavelet decomposition, soft thresholding of the coefficients at level 3, and then reconstruction. The onset time was defined based on the time of peak in the product of the first and the second derivatives of the optical signal. Starting at this time, each response was fitted (least-squares curve fitting) with a double-sigmoid function (a sigmoid rise followed by a sigmoid fall). The time of the peak of this response was defined as the peak in this fitted response function, rather than the peak of the raw optical signal.

To compare the temporal dynamics of ortho- vs. retronasal glomerular responses we extracted parameters of the time course of responses at each glomerulus. The parameters were first averaged across glomeruli for each odorant and route separately. Then the mean for each odorant per route was averaged across 9 rats. The temporal delay for the retronasal response relative to the corresponding orthonasal response is referred to as ‘retronasal delay’ or ‘retro delay’ in this paper.

Correlation analysis and ANOVA were used to establish whether the route of stimulation affected the temporal parameters, and how the ortho- vs. retronasal differences were affected by physicochemical properties of the odorants.

Measurement of temporal relationship between glomerular response and breathing

Our stimulus onset time was not triggered from the rats breathing cycle or other signals, but instead was intended to be randomly initiated. This could have biased our results, in the unlikely case where potential breathing related blood oxygenation level, blood volume or blood flow rate related noise was not randomly distributed between ortho- and retronasal trials. We recorded breathing signals from each experimental animal throughout the experiments. Using a Matlab script we determined points of interest relative to the inspiration just before the stimulus onset (first inspiration) and extracted the values from each individual trial based on the instantaneous breathing frequency at the time of stimulus onset. We then determined if peak response for each route of stimulation corresponded to certain phase of breathing. We also compared ortho- and retronasal breathing rate, stimulus onset time from the preceding inspiration, position of stimulus onset within a breath cycle (range: 0 to 1), peak response time from the preceding inspiration and number of breath cycles until peak response.

Results

1. Ortho- and retronasal glomerular response patterns largely overlap

We first investigated how similar the retro- and orthonasal glomerular response patterns are. Figure 2 shows an example of averaged (n = 3) odor maps in the OB at their peak response for orthonasal 4% 2-hexanone (2hex), orthonasal 4% methyl valerate (MV) and retronasal 4% MV. Responses of glomerulus 1 and 12 vary strongly across the three conditions. Glomerulus 1 responded mainly to orthonasal MV, much less to the retronasal stimulation thereof, and not at all to orthonasal 2hex, whereas glomerulus 12 responded strongest to orthonasal 2hex. The average responses (mean ± sem) for these three stimuli at sixteen glomeruli appear in Fig. 2E. Paired t-test between ortho- 2hex and ortho- MV responses revealed significant differences at nine glomeruli. Between ortho- and retronasal MV, however, responses were different significantly only at two glomeruli. Correlation analysis of the responses between ortho- MV and ortho- 2hex across all sixteen glomeruli showed a negative correlation (r = −0.61), while that between ortho- vs. retronasal MV exhibited a strong positive correlation (r = 0.74).

Figure 2
Ortho- and retronasal glomerular response patterns elicited by the same odor overlap but differ in terms of response size. A–E, an example from a rat: A, RLI image of the left bulb. B–D, averaged (n = 3) odor maps for ortho 4% 2-hexanone ...

Across all 9 rats for 6 odors and air the glomerular pattern correlation between routes was 0.38 ± 0.09 (mean ± sem; n = 7), which increased to 0.45 ± 01 (n = 6) after removing putative outlier tolualdehyde (r = −0.03). The pattern similarity between different odors presented via the same route was 0.27 ± 0.03 (n = 42 odor-odor combinations), which decreased to 0.22 ± 0.04 (n = 30) after removing tolualdehyde. Response patterns were more similar for the same odorants between routes than for the same route between odors (p < 0.05) but only after removing tolualdehyde, the retronasal responses of which did not relate to the orthonasal ones. This was substantiated by using multidimensional scaling (Fig. 3B) to explore the response pattern similarities across all stimuli at once. Stimulus pairs are linked by lines. Figure 3B shows that odors are clearly differentiated along the hypothetical "odorant" axis, and are also differentiated (but to a lesser degree) along the "route" axis. The dotted line separates 5 of the six odorants by route.

Figure 3
Comparison of ortho- vs. retronasal response magnitudes and patterns for different odorants. A. The averaged ortho- and retronasal responses are shown, as well as their ratio to their left (mean ± sem, n = 6~9 rats). The magnitude of retronasal ...

When we analyzed the similarity in response magnitudes between all ortho- and retronasal pairs in a single correlation this yielded an even higher correlation of r = 0.51 (893 pairs, Fig. 2G). We therefore conclude that ortho- vs. retronasal OB response patterns elicited by the same odorant differ less than response patterns elicited by two different odorants via the same route. In other words, odor route affects response patterns less than odorant identity.

To estimate the degree of overlap between ortho- and retronasal odor maps we arbitrarily set < 0.05% ΔF/F as noise and calculated for each stimulus the percentage of the total number of responsive glomeruli in an animal that was responsive to each route. On average (6 odorants, 9 rats, 779 glomerular responses) we found 69 ± 9% (mean ± sd) overlap between the two routes (Fig. 2F). Remaining 20 ± 4% and 11 ± 6% glomeruli were activated, respectively, by ortho- and retronasal stimulation only. In terms of the number of glomeruli imaged per rat, 11.14 ± 2.37 (mean ± sd) responded to both routes while 2.48 ± 0.48 and 1.51 ± 0.73 glomeruli responded only to ortho- and retronasal stimulation, respectively.

2. Retronasal response amplitudes are smaller than orthonasal ones

We next asked how response magnitudes were affected by the routes of odorant delivery. Figure 3A compares retronasal response magnitudes to the corresponding orthonasal responses across six odorants and clean air, ordered by decreasing polarity (octanol : water partition coefficient, kow). Fig. 3A shows both the mean glomerular response (first %ΔF/F averaged across glomeruli in each rat and then averaged across rats, mean ± sem; n = 5~9 rats) for each route and, the r/o ratio of %ΔF/F (averaged across rats; mean ± sem; n = 5~9) providing an estimate of the pair-wise retronasal response efficacy relative to the orthonasal response. Responses to the orthonasal 2-hexanone (2hex) (0.47 ± 0.05 %ΔF/F) were significantly larger than the retronasal responses (0.27 ± 0.10 %ΔF/F) (p < 0.05, r/o = 0.52 ± 0.19, n = 6). Ethyl butyrate (EB), showed relatively larger responses both ortho- (0.65 ± 0.07 %ΔF/F) and retronasally (0.37 ± 0.09 %ΔF/F), and the difference was significant (p < 0.05, r/o = 0.59 ± 0.14, n = 9). Methyl valerate (MV) responses were also robust orthonasally (0.60 ± 0.08 %ΔF/F) but less so retronasally (0.30 ± 0.08 %ΔF/F), showing a significant difference (p < 0.05, r/o = 0.61 ± 0.18, n = 8). Amyl acetate (AA) showed a significantly smaller (p < 0.05) retronasal response (0.14 ± 0.06 %ΔF/F) compared to an almost double orthonasal response (0.31 ± 0.04 %ΔF/F, r/o = 0.49 ± 0.18). Tolualdehyde (tolu) retronasal responses (0.10 ± 0.05 %ΔF/F) were also the smallest among the six odorants tested, and the orthonasal responses (0.26 ± 0.07 %ΔF/F) were significantly larger (p < 0.05, r/o = 0.34 ± 0.24, n = 5). Vinyl cyclohexane (VC), however, was only close to a significant difference in the response between the routes (ortho: 0.19 ± 0.05 %ΔF/F, retro: 0.11 ± 0.04 %ΔF/F, p = 0.07, r/o = 0.62 ± 0.20, n = 7). By contrast, clean air evoked responses of similar magnitude (ortho: 0.16 ± 0.03, retro: 0.13 ± 0.05 %ΔF/F, p = 0.22, r/o = 0.71 ± 0.36, n = 9). Across all ortho- vs. retronasal responses the orthonasal responses (0.39 ± 0.04 %ΔF/F) were significantly larger (p < 0.01, r/o = 0.63 ± 0.12, n = 9) than the retronasal responses (0.23 ± 0.04 %ΔF/F). The correlation analysis across all 893 ortho-retronasal stimulus pairs, shown in Fig. 2G, similarly suggests that retronasal response amplitudes were 52% of the orthonasal magnitudes, which is close to the 63% suggested by the ratio above. These results indicate that, in general, retronasal responses to odorant stimuli are significantly smaller than orthonasal responses, despite identical odor concentrations and flow rate.

3. Volatility enhances retronasal response efficacy

To determine if there was a relationship between specific physicochemical properties of the odorants and their retronasal response efficacy relative to orthonasal responses, we plotted the odors and these properties relative to the mean retro/ortho ratio. We found that the relative retronasal efficacy (r/o ratio) did not correlate well with the lipophilicity/partition coefficient (P (log kow), r = 0.21, p = 0.34, Fig. 4B) or polar surface area (polar surface (Å2), r = − 0.10, p = 0.42, Fig. 4E), but did correlate strongly with vapor pressure (VP (mmHg), r = 0.92, p < 0.005, Fig. 4A), density ((g/ml), r = − 0.80, p < 0.05, Fig. 4C) and boiling point (BP (°C), r = − 0.93, p < 0.005, Fig. 4D). The orthonasal and retronasal response magnitudes at best only trended toward significant correlations with these properties (no shown).The ortho-retro pattern similarity was found to increase only with decreasing boiling point (r2 = 0.88, n = 6, p < 0.005) and with decreasing density (r2 = 0.79, n = 6, p < 0.01), and additionally with retro/ortho response ratio (r2 = 0.68, n = 6, p < 0.05).

Figure 4
Retronasal relative response efficacy correlates with the volatility of the odorants. The mean retro/ortho response ratio of Fig. 3A for each odorant is plotted against vapor pressure (VP, mmHg; A), partition coefficient (P, logKow, B), density (g/ml, ...

We'd like to point out that these findings were robust and not dependent on a hinge-like effect of the single high density and high boiling point odorant (Fig. 4C and 4D), in that in a complimentary analysis based on correlation-derived slopes rather than ratios (Fig. 3A) the same results were obtained. There we also included two additional odorants, being the highly polar cyclohexane and highly non-polar mesitylene with continuous values for density and boiling point (0.95, 0.87 mg/ml, and 155°C and 164°C, respectively). In that analysis the relation between density and r/o ratio yielded r2 = 0.76 (p < 0.01) and the relation between boiling point and r/o ratio yielded r2 = 0.83 (p < 0.001). These two odors were otherwise excluded from analyses due to the low number of rats tested with them (4 rats).

As higher vapor pressure, lower density and lower boiling point tend to make an odorant more volatile, these results indicate that higher volatility increases especially retronasal efficacy and thereby increases the pattern similarity between the ortho- and retronasal response in the presynaptic dorsal OB.

4. Retronasal responses show slower but more consistent temporal dynamics than orthonasal responses

The temporal dynamics of calcium responses may contain information about odor quality at the level of the OB (Spors et al., 2006; Junek et al., 2010). Significant differences may exist between the response latencies of different glomeruli to the same odorant; and, for a given glomerulus, different odorants can evoke responses with different latencies (Spors et al., 2006). To compare the temporal dynamics of ortho- vs. retronasal glomerular responses we examined the time course of the responses of each glomerulus (see Materials and Methods, and Fig. 5A). The averaged values of each parameter for each odorant (ordered by polarity) and each route are shown separately in Fig. 5 (B&C). Temporal response parameters were relatively variable across orthonasally presented odorants, notably for t10 (F6,50 = 12.1, p < 10−7, one-way ANOVA), t50 (F6,50 = 3.4, p < 0.01) and rise-time (F6,50 = 6.4, p < 10−4). Retronasal dynamics barely varied between odorants, only for t50 (F6,50 = 3.3, p < 0.01). This is more clearly shown in Fig. 5E, which shows the reconstituted sigmoidal plots for orthonasal (red) and retronasal (blue) odorants.

Figure 5
Temporal dynamics of ortho- vs. retronasal odor responses. A, response traces were fitted with a double sigmoidal function and the time of response onset (start), time to 10%, 50% and 90% of the response peak (t10, t50 and t90), the time of the response ...

In order to determine if any of the temporal parameters differ between the ortho- and retronasal route we subtracted retro values from the ortho values for each odor and parameter in pair-wise fashion (Fig. 5D). We found that the average duration of the lag between the stimulus onset and the response onset (start) was −439 ± 30 ms for ortho and −550 ± 22 ms for retronasal responses with a resulting retronasal delay (ortho minus retro, "retro delay") of 100 ± 39 ms relative to the orthonasal response (p < 0.0001, n = 57 (7 odors × 9 rats, 6 missing data points), paired 2-tailed t-test). The retronasal route was associated with slightly faster response from start to 10% of peak amplitude (retro delay t10, 42 ± 11 ms, (p < 0.0001), Fig. 5D), but similar from start to 50% (t50, 11 ± 7 ms, (p = 0.20)). The retronasal responses were consistently slower to reach 90% of peak (t90, −45 ± 22 ms, (p = 0.002)) and peak itself (tpeak, −147 ± 47 ms, (p = 0.0001)). On average the rise-time for retronasal response was longer than that for orthonasal response by 169 ± 49 milliseconds (p < 0.0001). Unpaired two-sided t-tests yielded the same conclusions.

We also performed two-way ANOVA to understand possible effects of route, odor and their interactions on each of the temporal parameters. Route showed a significant (F1,100>7.6, p < 0.01) effect on each of the temporal parameters except for t50 (p = 0.35), consistent with the t-tests above. Likewise, across both routes odor had an effect (F6,100 > 2.4, p < 0.05) on t-onset, t10, t50, and rise-time but not on t90 (p = 0.41) and t-peak (p = 0.20). The interaction between route and odor was significant only for the rise-time (F6,100 = 2.5, p < 0.05). A one-way ANOVA of odor effect on retro delay only showed an effect on rise time (F5,60 = 2.9, p < 0.05), as expected based on the 2-way ANOVA, and also t90 (F5,60 = 2.3, p < 0.05).

It should be noted that the response latencies we report are on the long side, but are not outside the bounds of prior measures. Response onset delay was ~0.5 sec, of which ~120ms was due to dead space in the olfactometer (see Methods). The remaining ~380ms are not unusual, as similar delays have been reported before using EOG. For example, see Fig. 3 in (Scott, 2006), where the ventrolateral response to isoamyl acetate at 200ml/min flow rate takes 300–400ms to develop.

We also would like to point out that despite having minimized the difference the dead-space of our olfactometer between both routes, it is possible that within the rat itself differences in dead space remain (i.e. between the odor mask or retronasal canula and OE). These could have accounted for the difference in onset delay between routes (Fig 5D, E), but do not explain the effect of route, or polarity on the other parameters from which this delay was subtracted (t10,t50, t90, tpeak and rise-time, Fig 5, ,6).6). Indeed, we subtract onset delay from the other parameters and variability remains (Fig. 5B&C, "#") as tested by ANOVA.

Figure 6
Retronasal delay (inverted values from Fig. 5D for clarity) for the onset of the response and time to peak correlates with the polarity of odorants. A, mean retro delay for the onset of the response for each odorant is plotted against vapor pressure (VP), ...

Overall, these observations suggest that the time course of retronasal responses is slower than that of orthonasal responses, including a longer onset latency, longer time to peak and longer rise-time. Moreover, the response dynamics varied less across odors presented retronasally than orthonasally.

5. Retronasal delay for the onset, time to peak and rise-time varies with the polarity of the odorants

We then asked if and how retronasal delays of Fig. 5D correlated with specific odorant properties (Table 3). As shown in Fig. 6A mean retro delay (which values are the inverted values of Fig. 5D for clarity) for the onset of the response for each odorant was plotted against its partition coefficient (P), polar surface area (polar surface), vapor pressure (VP), density (d) and boiling point (BP). The response onset delay of the retronasal route versus the orthonasal route did not correlate with vapor pressure (r = 0.18, p = 0.37), density (r = 0.24, p = 0.32) or boiling point (r = −0.07, p = 0.45), but did correlate well with polar surface (r = 0.81, p < 0.05) and partition coefficient (r = −0.95, p < 0.005).

Table 3
Relationship between retro-delay and odorant properties.

Similarly, the mean retro delay for time to peak also correlated with partition coefficient (r = −0.93, p < 0.005; Fig. 6Bb). Again, polar surface showed a positive correlation but no clear significance (r = 0.59, p = 0.11). Vapor pressure (r = 0.16, p = 0.38), boiling point (r = −0.16, p = 0.38) and density (r = 0.06, p = 0.46) did not appear to explain the variation in time to peak.

These data indicate that odorant polarity plays an important role in the length of the delay of retronasal relative to orthonasal responses. Increasing polar surface by 1 unit increases the response onset time by 3.7 ms and time to reach peak of retronasal responses by 5.2 ms, relative to orthonasal responses. Increasing the odorant's polarity by 1 log unit (i.e. decreasing P) increases the relative retronasal onset by ~49 ms and the time to peak by ~192ms. Polarity also correlated positively with relative (retro delay) rise-time and t10 nearly significantly (p = 0.06 and p =0.08, respectively). Given the larger variability in the dynamics of responses to orthonsasal odors (Fig. 5), we suggest that these effects of polarity are largely on orthonasal responses.

6. Ruling out effects of phase of breathing cycle and stimulus pressure on ortho- vs. retronasal responses

We recorded breathing and intranasal pressure signals from each animal for each route throughout the experiments. We tested whether glomerular responses to ortho- and retronasal stimulation had any unexpected relationships with these parameters. We did not expect effects of breathing, as stimulus onset was timed independently thereof. The double tracheotomized rats were breathing through a tracheal tube, uncoupled from the nose. The breathing frequency in animals remained uniform throughout the experiment (ortho: 1.60 ± 0.11 Hz, retro: 1.65 ± 0.14 Hz) (Fig. 7). We compared stimulus onset time from the preceding inspiration (ortho: 0.31 ± 0.02 s, retro: 0.31 ± 0.01 s), position of stimulus onset within a breath cycle (ortho: 0.47 ± 0.02, retro: 0.47 ± 0.01), range: 0 to 1), peak response time from the preceding inspiration (ortho: 1.61 ± 0.12 s, retro: 1.62 ± 0.12 s) and number of breath cycles until peak response (ortho: 2.56 ± 0.25, retro: 2.66 ± 0.31). As expected, there was no significant difference between ortho- and retronasal trials on these parameters, suggesting that the observed effects of route on glomerular responses were not influenced by the phase of breathing cycle on average.

Figure 7
No coupling of ortho and retronasal olfactory glomerular responses with breathing. A, an example of the time course of a glomerular response, breathing and artificial sniffing. Points of interest are marked with a, b, c and d. B, comparison of ortho and ...

We also did not expect differences in intranasal pressure responses to ortho- and retronasal trials. Indeed, the difference in the intranasal pressure parameters (maximum pressure (Pmax), area under the pressure curve during odor stimulation (Pauc) and the time to Pmax could not explain the effect of route on the relative magnitudes of responses (Fig. 8). The ratios of paired averaged ortho- and retronasal responses for each stimulus for all animals ("r/o dF/F"; n = 106) did not correlate with averaged pressure ratios based on the same trials: Pmax explained 1%, Pauc 3% and time to Pmax 1% (Fig. 8). On average Pmax and Pauc were somewhat below unity (0.90 ± 0.01), indicating that the retronasal flow resistance was 10% higher than the orthonasal flow. No temporal differences in flow were found.

Figure 8
No correlation between intranasal pressure (flow-resistance) during artificial sniffing and effect of route on glomerular responses. A, scatter plot of retro/ortho response ratio vs. ortho/retro ratio of maximum pressure during stimulus delivery. B, scatter ...

Discussion

The spatial and temporal pattern of OB glomerular activity evoked by odorants is believed to represent all information about those odorants. In the present study, we investigated the effect of route on both spatial and temporal presynaptic glomerular response patterns in a rat model. To provide a mechanistic insight into the phenomena, we further examined the effects of physicochemical properties of the odorants on the ortho- versus retronasal differences.

1. Gross spatial activation patterns are largely independent of stimulus route

Four aspects of the glomerular activity patterns have been proposed to contribute to an early code of odor quality in the brain: (a) the gross binary spatial glomerular response pattern, (b) the (relative) pattern of the glomerular response amplitudes, (c) the temporal dynamics and (d) the responses relative to the phase of the respiratory cycle (Spors et al., 2006; Carey et al., 2009). To determine if change of route would alter the glomerular response patterns we explored each of these dimensions. First we examined the gross (combinatorial) glomerular response patterns for each odorant stimulus and arrived at one of the basic findings of this study that retronasal stimulation did not activate a distinct set of glomeruli. Even though the magnitudes of retronasal responses were generally smaller than the orthonasal counterparts, the glomeruli activated by retronasal stimulation were not substantially different from those activated by the corresponding orthonasal stimulation (e.g., Figs. 2 and 3A).3A). Even when tiny responses (< 0.05% ΔF/F) were considered as noise, more than two-third of the activated glomeruli were still common to both of the routes. Hence, we conclude that retronasal responses consist of largely the same set of activated glomeruli as orthonasal responses (Fig. 2G), and that the response differences between the routes are found elsewhere. Only one caveat to this conclusion is that since we were looking only at the dorsal part of the OB, recruitment of glomeruli could occur in other areas of the OB in a route-dependent manner.

2. Retronasal OB responses tend to be smaller than orthonasal responses

Next, we looked into the pattern of response magnitudes across glomeruli and found that retronasal responses generally correlated positively with the orthonasal responses, but were clearly smaller in magnitude, 52% when compared pair-wise (Fig. 2G) or 63% when compared ratio-wise (Fig. 3A).

If the response amplitude were the only difference between the two routes, it would raise the question whether retronasal responses are merely equivalent to the orthonasal response to a lower concentration of the same odor. This was not the case because further analysis revealed additional differences. For example, the retro- versus orthonasal pattern similarity and efficacy varied across odorants and correlated only with vapor pressure, density and boiling point, but not polarity/partition coefficient and polar surface (Fig. 4). This would not have been the case if retronasal responses were simply like weaker orthonasal responses.

The significant odorant-specific effects of vapor pressure, density and boiling point on the relative magnitude of retronasal responses suggest that the more volatile the odorants are, the larger the relative retronasal responses to them tend to be. Simulations of air flow in the nasal cavity of rodents and humans suggest that retronasal flow is less effective at engaging the olfactory epithelium, especially in the caudo-dorsal area of the nasal cavity (Zhao et al., 2004; Zhao et al., 2006). Thus it is likely that a more volatile odorant may access the caudodorsal olfactory area in the nasal cavity more effectively.

Interestingly, the factors affecting odorant sorption (polarity and polar surface) did not have a significant effect on the relative retronasal response magnitudes, but did, and nearly exclusively so, on the relative temporal dynamics (see below).

3. Retronasal temporal delay: longer response onset latency and slower rise

Reported temporal analyses of odorant-evoked (orthonasal) input to the dorsal OB in rodents indicate that odorants can evoke diverse temporal patterns across activated glomeruli in an odorant-specific manner (Spors et al., 2006; Junek et al., 2010). Here we observed that retronasal responses generally have a longer latency to onset and peak than orthonasal responses. To be sure, not only are retronasal responses slower to start, but also take more time to reach their peak level relative to this response onset (Fig. 5E). Thus, the retronasal time-courses were not simply uniformly delayed (i.e. was shifted rightwards, Fig. 5E), but were overall more sluggish and less robust.

These results cannot merely be the effect of ‘weaker’ stimulation, because it has been shown that onset latency to orthonasal responses does not vary with odorant concentration (0.2~5% s.v. of 2-hexanone), and there is also no consistent relationship between response latency and response amplitude (see Fig. 2D in (Spors et al., 2006)).

Whereas volatility (but not polarity) appeared to enhance relative retronasal efficacy, polarity (but not volatility) made the relative retronasal response more sluggish. Thus, the relative retronasal delay for the onset latency and peak response time varied across odorants and strongly correlated with odorant polarity, but odor volatility (Fig. 6 and Table 3).

The significant effects of odorant polarity and polar surface that we observed on the temporal dynamics of ortho- versus retronasal responses across odorants are consistent with published literature on sorption. Odorant polarity has been recognized as a potential factor affecting dynamics of the odorant sorption in the olfactory epithelium (Mozell and Jagodowicz, 1973; Mozell et al., 1984; Palm et al., 1997; Kelder et al., 1999). Effects of odorant polar surface area on olfactory responses has not been reported yet, but polar surface properties have been reported to be predictive of intestinal absorption of drugs in humans (Palm et al., 1997; Krarup et al., 1998), and also a dominant determinant for oral absorption and brain penetration of drugs (Kelder et al., 1999).

Hence our data indicate that relatively ‘slower’ retronasal response can be largely attributed to the odorant sorption across the olfactory epithelium. That is, the gas chromatography model of the olfactory epithelium fits nicely with the onset latencies we observed. Increasing the polarity of the odorants tended to display longer retronasal delays of 100–300 ms for the onset time and peak response time, consistent with the idea of more efficient delivery of hydrophobic compared to hydrophilic odorants in retronasal stimulation of dorsolateral ORNs in vivo (Scott et al., 2007). Our data further show that even though hydrophilic odorants reach ORNs relatively late, this delay does not affect the response magnitude.

4. Controls: no effects of phase of breathing cycle and flow resistance

Several reports on the orthonasal OB responses in free breathing rodents have shown coupling of glomerular responses with sniffing, and suggested that glomerular responses were modulated by breathing (Chaput et al., 1992; Spors et al., 2006). In our double tracheotomized artificially sniffing rats, where breathing and odor stimulation are uncoupled, ortho- and retronasal stimulus onsets, as well as their responses, occurred on average at the same time relative to the breathing cycle (Fig. 7), implying that the observed effects of route on presynaptic glomerular responses were not due to unintended respiratory modulation.

Our setup was designed to minimize route-dependent differences between the stimuli. For example, odorant flows were generated by the same olfactometer irrespective of flow direction. Stimulus onset delays were minimized by volumetrically matching the dead space of each route. We could, however, not rule out biological factors. Indeed we did find that the retronasal flow resistance was 10% higher than the orthonasal resistance (Fig. 8D), due entirely to the flow paths inside the animal (i.e., it did not occur when we bypassed the rats). Clearly, the retronasal route has a mild flow-rectifying component. We found no evidence that this biased relative response magnitudes across the entirety of our dataset (Fig. 8A–C)

5. Encoding of odor route

Data presented in this study have generated an interesting question: what biological activity of the bulb, if any, encodes the information about the odor route? It does appear that odor route can be sensed per se by humans (Small et al., 2005). We do not think route information is mediated by a subset of individual glomeruli, as we did not typically see glomeruli ‘dedicated’ to a particular route (glomerulus 8 in Fig. 2E is rather exceptional in that respect). By contrast, we propose that information about odor route is contained in the spatio-temporal response patterns across the entire OB. Currently we do not know what behavior (e.g., swallow or breathing) could serve as the reference for the onset latency, and if such a reference is a prerequisite to perceptual discrimination of the direction of odor flow. It will be of interest to evaluate the perceptual effects of varying odorant volatility versus varying odorant polarity, here proposed to rather selectively affect the relative retronasal response magnitude and relative retronasal response lag, respectively. This may have important consequences for understanding the perception and neural encoding of food flavor, which is important for feeding behavior and health (Shepherd, 2012).

Acknowledgements

We'd like to thank Ryan M. Carey for kindly sharing the temporal sigmoid fitting Matlab code. We'd further like to thank Drs. B. A. Johnson and M. Leon for access to and discussion of their raw data files for Matlab evaluation. We'd like to thank the John B. Pierce Laboratory machine shop (John Buckley, Michael Fritz, Ron Goodman, Tom D'Alessandro and Angelo DiRubba) for their excellent technical support. We thank Larry Marks, David Willhite, Michelle Rebello and Tom McTavish for valuable feedback on an earlier version of the manuscript.

This work is supported by NIH/NIDCD grants R01DC009994 and R01DC011286.

References

  • Belluscio L, Katz LC. Symmetry, stereotypy, and topography of odorant representations in mouse olfactory bulbs. J Neurosci. 2001;21:2113–2122. [PubMed]
  • Bozza T, McGann JP, Mombaerts P, Wachowiak M. In vivo imaging of neuronal activity by targeted expression of a genetically encoded probe in the mouse. Neuron. 2004;42:9–21. [PubMed]
  • Carey RM, Verhagen JV, Wesson DW, PÃ-rez Ns, Wachowiak M. Temporal Structure of Receptor Neuron Input to the Olfactory Bulb Imaged in Behaving Rats. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2009;101:1073–1088. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Chaput MA, Buonviso N, Berthommier F. Temporal patterns in spontaneous and odour-evoked mitral cell discharges recorded in anaesthetized freely breathing animals. Eur J Neurosci. 1992;4:813–822. [PubMed]
  • Heilmann S, Hummel T. A new method for comparing orthonasal and retronasal olfaction. Behav Neurosci. 2004;118:412–419. [PubMed]
  • Johnson BA, Leon M. Modular representations of odorants in the glomerular layer of the rat olfactory bulb and the effects of stimulus concentration. J Comp Neurol. 2000;422:496–509. [PubMed]
  • Johnson BA, Leon M. Chemotopic odorant coding in a mammalian olfactory system. J Comp Neur. 2007;503:1–34. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Junek S, Kludt E, Wolf F, Schild D. Olfactory Coding with Patterns of Response Latencies. Neuron. 2010;67:872–884. [PubMed]
  • Kelder J, Grootenhuis PDJ, Bayada DM, Delbressine LPC, Ploemen JP. Polar Molecular Surface as a Dominating Determinant for Oral Absorption and Brain Penetration of Drugs. Pharmaceutical Research. 1999;16:1514–1519. [PubMed]
  • Krarup LH, Christensen IT, Hovgaard L, Frokjaer S. Predicting Drug Absorption from Molecular Surface Properties Based on Molecular Dynamics Simulations. Pharmaceutical Research. 1998;15:972–978. [PubMed]
  • Lam YW, Cohen LB, Wachowiak M, Zochowski MR. Odors elicit three different oscillations in the turtle olfactory bulb. J Neurosci. 2000;20:749–762. [PubMed]
  • Meister M, Bonhoeffer T. Tuning and topography in an odor map on the rat olfactory bulb. J Neurosci. 2001;21:1351–1360. [PubMed]
  • Mori K, Nagao H, Yoshihara Y. The olfactory bulb: coding and processing of odor molecule information. Science. 1999;286:711–715. [PubMed]
  • Mozell MM, Jagodowicz M. Chromatographic separation of odorants by the nose: retention times measured across in vivo olfactory mucosa. Science. 1973;181:1247–1249. [PubMed]
  • Mozell MM, Sheehe PR, Swieck SW, Jr, Kurtz DB, Hornung DE. A parametric study of the stimulation variables affecting the magnitude of the olfactory nerve response. J Gen Physiol. 1984;83:233–267. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Palm K, Stenberg P, Luthman K, Artursson P. Polar Molecular Surface Properties Predict the Intestinal Absoption of Drugs in Humans. Pharmaceutical Research. 1997;14:568–571. [PubMed]
  • Pierce J, Halpern BP. Orthonasal and retronasal odorant identification based upon vapor phase input from common substances. Chem Senses. 1996;21:529–543. [PubMed]
  • Ratzlaff EH, Grinvald A. A tandem-lems epifluorescence macroscope: hundred-fold brightness advantage for wide-field imaging. J Neurosci Methods. 1991;36:127–137. [PubMed]
  • Ressler KJ, Sullivan SL, Buck LB. A zonal organization of odorant receptor gene expression in the olfactory epithelium. Cell. 1993;73:597–609. [PubMed]
  • Rozin P. "Taste-smell confusions" and the duality of the olfactory sense. Percept & Psychophys. 1982;31:397–401. [PubMed]
  • Scott-Johnson PE, Blakley D, Scott JW. Effects of air flow on rat electroolfactogram. Chem Senses. 2000;25:761–768. [PubMed]
  • Scott JW. Sniffing and temporal coding in olfaction. Chem Senses. 2006;31:119–130. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Scott JW, Acevedo HP, Sherrill L, Phan M. Responses of the rat olfactory epithelium to retronasal air flow. J Neurophys. 2007;97:1941–1950. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Shepherd GM. How the brain creates flavor and why it matters. 1st Edition. New York: Columbia University Press; 2012. Neurogastronomy.
  • Small DM, Gerber JC, Mak YE, Hummel T. Differential neural responses evoked by orthonasal versus retronasal odorant perception in humans. Neuron. 2005;47:593–605. [PubMed]
  • Spors H, Wachowiak M, Cohen LB, Friedrich RW. Temporal dynamics and latency patterns of receptor neuron input to the olfactory bulb. J Neurosci. 2006;26:1247–1259. [PubMed]
  • Sun BC, Halpern BP. Identification of air phase retronasal and orthonasal odorant pairs. Chem Senses. 2005;30:693–706. [PubMed]
  • Uchida N, Takahashi YK, Tanifuji M, Mori K. Odor maps in the mammalian olfactory bulb: domain organization and odorant structural features. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1035–1043. [PubMed]
  • Vassar R, Ngai J, Axel R. Spatial segregation of odorant receptor expression in the mammalian olfactory epithelium. Cell. 1993;74:309–318. [PubMed]
  • Verhagen JV, Wesson DW, Netoff TI, White JA, Wachowiak M. Sniffing controls an adaptive filter of sensory input to the olfactory bulb. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10:631–639. [PubMed]
  • Wachowiak M, Cohen LB. Representation of odorants by receptor neuron input to the mouse olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2001;32:723–735. [PubMed]
  • Wachowiak M, Denk W, Friedrich RW. Functional organization of sensory input to the olfactory bulb glomerulus analyzed by two-photon calcium imaging. PNAS. 2004;101:9097–9102. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Wesson DW, Carey RM, Verhagen JV, Wachowiak M. Rapid encoding and perception of novel odors in the rat. PLOS biology. 2008;6:e82. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Xu F, Liu N, Kida I, Rothman DL, Hyder F, Shepherd GM. Odor maps of aldehydes and esters revealed by functional MRI in the glomerular layer of the mouse olfactory bulb. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100:11029–11034. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Youngentob SL, Mozell MM, Sheehe PR, Hornung DE. A quantitative analysis of sniffing strategies in rats performing odor detection tasks. Physiol Behav. 1987;41:59–69. [PubMed]
  • Zhao K, Scherer PW, Hajiloo SA, Dalton P. Effect of anatomy on human nasal air flow and odor transport pattern:implications for olfaction. Chem Senses. 2004;29:365–379. [PubMed]
  • Zhao K, Dalton P, Yang GC, Scherer PW. Numerical modeling of turbulent and laminar airflow and odorant transport during sniffing in the human and rat nose. Chem Senses. 2006;31:107–118. [PubMed]
PubReader format: click here to try

Formats:

Related citations in PubMed

See reviews...See all...

Cited by other articles in PMC

See all...

Links

Recent Activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...