• We are sorry, but NCBI web applications do not support your browser and may not function properly. More information
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC Aug 25, 2010.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2928227
NIHMSID: NIHMS126134

How the optic nerve allocates space, energy capacity, and information

Abstract

Fiber tracts should use space and energy efficiently because both resources constrain neural computation. We found for a myelinated tract (optic nerve) that astrocytes use nearly 30% of the space and more than 70% of the mitochondria, establishing the significance of astrocytes for the brain’s space and energy budgets. Axons are mostly thin with a skewed distribution peaking at 0.7µm, near the lower limit set by channel noise. This distribution is matched closely by the distribution of mean firing rates measured under naturalistic conditions, suggesting that firing rate increases proportionally with axon diameter. In axons thicker than 0.7µm mitochondria occupy a constant fraction of axonal volume -- thus, mitochondrial volumes rise as the diameter squared. These results imply a law of diminishing returns: twice the information rate requires more than twice the space and energy capacity. We conclude that the optic nerve conserves space and energy by sending most information at low rates over fine axons with small terminal arbors, and sending some information at higher rates over thicker axons with larger terminal arbors – but only where more bits/s are needed for a specific purpose. Thicker axons seem to be needed, not for their greater conduction velocity (nor other intrinsic electrophysiological purpose), but instead to support larger terminal arbors and more active zones that transfer information synaptically at higher rates.

Keywords: mitochondria, EM, optic nerve, energy, information, Guinea pig

INTRODUCTION

Axons in the mammalian central nervous system exhibit a broad range of diameters. The thinnest axons, which arise from the smallest neurons, are about 0.2 µm, approaching the limit set by channel noise (Faisal et al., 2005). The thickest axons, which arise from the largest neurons, reach about 20 µm. In many tracts the distribution of axon diameters is highly skewed, such that thin axons are numerous and thick ones are rare (e.g., Biedenbach et al., 1986; Wang et al., 2008). This distribution, a basic feature of brain structure, has never been explained.

The standard explanation for thick axons is that their higher conduction velocities are needed to reduce conduction times (Hursh, 1939, Rushton, 1951; Waxman and Bennett, 1972; Koch, 1999). For example, Ia fibers conduct impulses from muscle spindles up to 120 m/s (Hunt, 1954; Eccles et al., 1962). This speed is essential because Ia axons form the afferent limb of a feedback loop with a long conduction distance. Slow conduction would cause long conduction times, so error messages would always be outdated. Yet were it generally true that conduction time sets axon diameter, thick fibers might predominate in long tracts and thin fibers in short ones, but this is not so (Friede et al., 1984). Furthermore, even if the increased conduction velocity could explain why certain axons need to be thick, it does not explain why mostly they are thin.

One obvious need is to conserve space: fiber volume increases as the radius squared, so the volume occupied by the thickest axon could accommodate 10,000 of the thinnest ones. Therefore, a brain with mostly thin axons can make more connections (Wen and Chklovskii, 2005). Another need might be to save energy: action potentials may consume up to 35% of the cortical energy budget (Attwell and Laughlin, 2001; Laughlin and Sejnowski, 2003; Lennie, 2003). So, if spiking by a thick axon uses more energy, a brain with mostly thin axons could afford more spikes. These considerations led us to quantify how space, energy capacity, and firing rate are related in a particular central tract, the optic nerve. We studied guinea pig because in this species the spike rates to naturalistic stimuli were known for the smallest and largest ganglion cell types (Koch et al., 2006); thus we hoped with further recordings to relate fiber size to information rate.

First we asked: how is space apportioned in the optic nerve between axons versus glia and between axons of different caliber? Second, how are mitochondria apportioned: across cell types (axons vs. glia), across subcellular compartments (e.g., nodes of Ranvier), across axons of different caliber, and across myelinated and unmyelinated regions of the same axons? Finally we asked: how does the distribution of ganglion cell firing rates (and thus information rates) correspond to the distributions of fiber size and oxidative capacity? We found that optic axons, like fly photoreceptors (Niven et al., 2007), follow a law of diminishing returns: twice the information rate requires more than twice the space and energy capacity.

METHODS

Electron microscopy

Retinas and optic nerves were prepared from three adult male guinea pigs (400–500 g). An animal was anesthetized with ketamine (100 mg/kg), xylazine (20 mg/kg) and pentobarbital (50 mg/kg) and then perfused with 2% paraformaldehyde + 2% glutaraldehyde in 0.1M phosphate buffer. Tissue was stored overnight at 4 deg °C and then dissected, osmicated, soaked in uranyl acetate, and prepared for electron microscopy as previously described (Tsukamoto et al., 1992). The method of fixation followed here results in no measurable tissue shrinkage (Tsukamoto et al., 1992). Electron micrographs were taken at 5000 times magnification and digitized.

Data analysis

Axonal profiles were identified by custom software written in MATLAB. Mitochondrial identification and the correction of misidentified axons (~5% of the profiles) were done manually using Adobe Photoshop 8. Because axon profiles are not precisely circular, we measured the area of each profile and then calculated the diameter of the equivalent circle.

If this method were applied to an axon sectioned obliquely, the elliptical profile would produce a spuriously large diameter. However, this effect would be small. Considering a perfectly cylindrical axon, a section deviating by 20º from perpendicular would overestimate the diameter by only 2.8 %. For the most common axon (0.7 µm) the error would amount to 0.02 µm, and for the thickest axon (3.5 µm), the error would amount to less than 0.1 µm. Also, because the axons run parallel, obliquely sectioned axons would produce elliptical profiles with the same orientation. Yet, the distribution of orientations among 1400 axons was flat. This indicates that the axons were cut perpendicular to their long axes.

To calculate average mitochondrial volumes we pooled axons into bins that were separated by at least 0.1 µm and adjusted to contain at least 20 axons. Since the largest axons were sparse, this “adaptive binning” led to larger bins for the widest diameters (Fig 4). Because the mitochondrial concentration was approximately constant for large axon diameters, we knew that mitochondrial volume would be well described by a quadratic function of diameter. Thus we fit the mitochondrial volume to a general quadratic function of diameter parameterized as: Vm = A (d – s) [(d – s) + B] where the shift s is meant to reflect the absence of very small axons, perhaps due to their unreliable spike timing (Faisal and Laughlin, 2007).

Figure 4
Energy capacity vs. axon caliber

Measurements of ganglion cell firing rates

In vitro guinea pig retinas were prepared for extracellular recordings as previously described (Koch et al., 2004). A variety of naturalistic movies (optic flow, saccades etc.) were used for visual stimulation (Koch et al., 2006). Movies were displayed on a 1-inch CRT monitor with a P43 phosphor (545 nm, 640 × 480 pixels, 60 Hz frame rate, projected through the top port of the microscope, and focused onto the retina at a magnification of 7.6. Ganglion cells were recorded using a multielectrode array (Segev et al., 2004), or a loose patch electrode (Koch et al., 2004) and cell types were identified by spike responses and morphology.

For all types of guinea pig retinal ganglion cells, and all naturalistic stimuli, mean firing rates (Hz) have been related to information by the formula

I=0.26[(rδ)log2(rδ)+(1rδ)log2(1rδ)]/δ
(1)

were δ = 5ms is a time bin used to relate firing rates to firing probabilities and 0.26 represents the net loss of information to noise and correlations in the single cell response(Koch et al., 2006). We used this formula to convert measured mean firing rates into estimated information rates.

Fits to diameter and rate distributions

The axon diameter and firing rate distributions were fit to lognormal functions,

y=(A/2πσx)exp(ln(x/xc)2/2σ2),
(2)

by minimizing the square deviation of the data from the function (Fig 1E, Fig 5B, Fig 6B). Here A reflects the area under the data histogram, xc is the mean value of the natural logarithm of the independent variable x (axon diameter or firing rate), and σ is the standard deviation of ln(x). To carry out the fits axon diameters were binned at 0.1 microns while firing rates were binned at 1Hz.

Figure 1
Retinal ganglion cell axons are mostly thin
Figure 5
Unmyelinated axons and retinal glia
Figure 6
Retinal ganglion cells mostly fire at low rates

We had reason to expect that the highest firing rates came from cells with the largest axons and that the lowest rates were associated to the thinnest axons. To test a possible linear relationship between rate and diameter (R = K (d –s)) we applied this linear transformation to the independent variable (x) in lognormal curve that described the diameter distribution, and searched for the values of K and s that minimized the squared error of this transformed curve from the rate distribution data. We similarly tested a possible quadratic relation -- R = K2 (d−s)2 + K1 (d−s) -- between rate and diameter as well as a possible linear relation between the logarithms of the rate and the diameter (log(R) = log(C) + E log(d), or, equivalently, R = C dE). We tried the latter relation because any two lognormal curves can be transformed into each other by shifting the mean and rescaling the standard deviation of the logarithm of the independent variable (x) – i.e., by a linear transformation on log(x). We used this observation to directly derive the constants C and E that cause the rate and diameter lognormal fits to coincide.

Confocal microscopy

To investigate mitochondrial distribution at the light microscopic level, we used the mitochondria specific fluorescent dye tetramethylrhodamine ethyl ester (TMRE). The dye specifically stains live mitochondria by accumulating in the mitochondrion in proportion to membrane potential (Scaduto and Grotyohann, 1999). In vitro guinea pig retinal preparation was loaded with 50 nM TMRE dissolved in Ames’ bathing solution for 15 minutes. Images were acquired with an Olympus BX61WI confocal laser scanning microscope (Olympus America Inc., Melville, NY) under a 60X water immersion objective (NA 0.9) and Rhodamine RedX setting.

Calculation of Na+ accumulation based on a multi-compartment model

Computer simulations of impulse propagation and Na+ accumulation were based on compartmental models of myelinated and unmyelinated axons using a version of a multi-channel model (Fohlmeister and Miller, 1997a, b) modified to work at 37 ˚C (Fohlmeister, 2009 and personal communication ; see supplementary material). A key improvement for this study was that the Na current was evaluated as a permeability change - with each iteration of the simulation (0.025–0.00025 ms), [Na]i and I Na was re-evaluated based on Na entering the compartment. The rate of Na diffusion to the flanking internodal compartments was set by a single time constant. Each iteration of the Na change used the new Na value as the driving force for the Na current. Table 1 in the supplementary material summarizes the model parameters after tuning I Na and I K to obtain approximately 100 mV response amplitudes and realistic shapes of action potentials without signs of aberrant impulse peak broadening. Action potential shapes and conduction velocities were robust to changes in parameters – e.g., a 25% change in the sodium conductance only changes the peak heights and the velocities by a few percent. The model was formulated in the NEURON simulation environment (Hines and Carnevale, 1997). We specifically focused on the range of spike rates observed in previous studies (Koch et al., 2006) to determine whether their fidelity required the observed range of axon diameters.

Calculation of Na+ accumulation based on membrane capacitance

We also estimated Na+ influx for an action potential by the amount of charge required for a voltage change, ΔV = 100 mV, assuming a membrane capacitance, Cm = 1 µF/cm2, for a unit length axon segment (Attwell and Laughlin, 2001). We increased our estimates by 4-fold to take account of simultaneous activation of Na+ and K+ channels (Hodgkin, 1975). In unmyelinated axons, capacitance, therefore Na+ influx estimates were proportional to surface area.

In myelinated axons we set the nodal length constant (2 µm), and defined internodal distance and myelin thickness in proportion to axon diameter (Rushton, 1951). Since capacitance is inversely proportional to the number of membrane layers, myelin capacitance was Cmyelin=Cm/nlayers. To calculate the number of membrane layers, we assumed a membrane diameter of 5 nm (as we confirmed by EM), and used the known relationship between myelin thickness and axon diameter (Williams and Chalupa, 1983; Guy et al., 1989), thus nlayers = dmyelin/dmembrane. Larger diameter resulted in fewer nodes of Ranvier and thicker myelin sheaths, keeping the total membrane capacitance unchanged. These estimates of Na+ influx were consistent with the results of the multi-compartment model.

RESULTS

How the optic nerve allocates space

We quantified how space is apportioned in a cross-section through the nerve by identifying and measuring all the processes in a montage of electron micrographs covering ~3300 µm2 (Fig 1A–C). Axoplasm (1480 axons) occupied 32% of the total area. Every axon was myelinated, the number of wraps forming a constant proportion of axon diameter (Williams and Chalupa, 1983,Friede and Beuche, 1985,Guy et al., 1989). Myelin accounted for 25% of the nerve’s cross-section, so the axons plus their sheaths occupied 57 %.

The axons were separated by astrocyte processes (pale, with bundles of 10 nm filaments) and by oligodendrocyte processes (dark, with microtubules but not filaments). Astrocyte processes occupied 12% of the area and their somas occupied 16%; oligodendrocyte processes occupied 8% of the area and their somas occupied 5%. Extracellular space was 2%. Our sample did not include microglia (Peters et al., 1991). Of course, astrocytes are a common constituent of central tracts, but it surprised us to learn that in the optic nerve they occupy such a large fraction (nearly 30 %) of the total cross-section (Fig. 1C).

We then measured the axon inner diameters. The nerve cross-section was systematically sampled along a transect through its center. Included were 23 patches, each with ~1200 fibers, for a total of about 28,000 fibers. We found no evidence for topographic variation in the distribution of diameters, confirming Guy et al. (1989). In further agreement we calculated that the whole nerve contained about 100,000 fibers.

The narrowest profiles were ~ 0.2 µm diameter, and the widest were ~3.5 µm. This suggested that axon caliber varies by 17.5-fold and that volume (proportional to r2) varies by ~300-fold. We checked this by reconstructing a patch of ~1200 axons from electron micrographs of serial sections and found that over a distance of 12 µm, individual axons vary considerably in caliber - by a factor of ~2 (Fig. 1E, inset). Thus the finest profiles in a cross-section actually represent constrictions in axons of larger mean diameter (Fig 1D).

The distribution of axon diameters rose steeply, peaking at ~ 0.7 µm. The distribution was strongly skewed (Fig 1E). Correcting for the significant variation in caliber of individual axons (Fig. 1E, inset) slightly narrowed the axon diameter distribution at both ends but hardly changed its overall shape. The distribution was well described by a lognormal function (see Methods), i.e. the natural logarithm of the diameter was distributed as a Gaussian, and chi-squared error (assuming standard error for bins with N counts) was minimized when the Gaussian had a mean of log[0.86] and a standard deviation of 0.30. The parameters were well-constrained -- a 10% change more than doubled the chi-squared error. The diameter range containing 95% percent of the axons was 0.5 µm – 1.5 µm; thus, the ratio of the volumes varied by ~10-fold.

How the optic nerve allocates energy capacity

Astrocytes are supposed to split glucose anaerobically to lactate and shuttle it to neuronal mitochondria for oxidation (Pellerin and Magistretti, 1994,Poitry-Yamate et al., 1995). Nevertheless, astrocytes in the optic nerve contain abundant mitochondria (Fig 1B). In fact, mitochondrial volume in astrocytes was 3.2% of cytoplasmic volume, exceeding by nearly 2-fold their volume fraction in axoplasm (see below). Astrocytes contained about 72% of the optic nerve’s mitochondria. These mitochondria displayed a dense matrix and relatively little inner membrane structure, which apparently reflects a lower oxidation rate (Hackenbrock, 1968a,Hackenbrock, 1968b).

Axonal mitochondria were cigar-shaped and oriented roughly parallel with the axon’s long axis (Fig 2A). Measured from serial, longitudinal sections, they were 2.9 ± 2.3 µm long (n=24); measured from cross-sections they were 0.22 ± 0.05 µm wide (n=7100). Mitochondrial size was roughly constant across axon diameters. Based on mitochondrial size and total volume, we estimated that the mean separation between mitochondria along an axon (between their midpoints) was 3.2 µm. Given their mean lengths (nearly 3 µm), the mitochondria distributed fairly evenly along the axons, and no point in the axon could be even as much as one micron from an intracellular fountain of ATP (Fig 2B).

Figure 2
Energy capacity distributes evenly along the axon

The narrowest axonal profiles, now recognized as constrictions, never contained a mitochondrion. Furthermore, as a fine axon re-expanded from its narrowest point, the probability of finding a mitochondrion increased smoothly, reaching the mean value for all axons within ~2.5 µm. Profiles narrower than 0.6 µm at the constriction showed this behavior, but wider profiles, showed a constant probability (Fig 2B).

The slenderest mitochondria were slightly thicker than the constrictions and were absent from them. Immediately beyond a constriction, mitochondrial concentration (mitochondrial volume per unit axoplasmic volume) rose sharply, so the center of a constriction would be not much more than one micron from a mitochondrion (Fig 2B). In thicker axons, where the narrowest regions could accommodate a mitochondrion with room to spare, their concentration was essentially constant (Fig 2B). This suggests that the constrictions exist in vivo and that mitochondria simply wriggle through them on their anterograde and retrograde journeys.

To test whether mitochondrial volume, our primary measurement, reasonably estimates the distribution of oxidative capacity, we plotted volume versus area of the inner mitochondrial membrane - which bears the terminal elements of the respiratory cascade at fixed density (Weibel, 2000). Longitudinal slices through axonal mitochondria showed the inner membrane evaginating to form a convoluted mass of tubules that expand its surface area (Fig 2C). Where the inner membrane was seen on edge, forming a sharp image, it was traced to obtain the total length. Depth was estimated as the section thickness (~90 nm). Because the tubules showed no particular orientation, their partial areas are unbiased reflections of the total tubular surface area. Thus, each slice provided a measure of inner membrane area, and this correlated strongly with the volume of the slice (Fig 2D). Consequently, mitochondrial volume predicts oxidative capacity. Thus, in the remainder of this paper we take mitochondrial volume as a measure of energy capacity, i.e., the power or energy per unit time.

Mitochondria avoid nodes of Ranvier

Given that nodes of Ranvier, plus their flanking paranodes, are major sites of cation flux during an action potential (Scherer, 1999), we had expected to find an increased concentration of mitochondria. However, analyzing 51 nodes of Ranvier, we found that mitochondria, far from concentrating at nodes, actually avoided them (Fig 3A, C). The distance from the center of a node to the nearest mitochondrion was about 3 µm, and by about 4µm mitochondria reached their mean axoplasmic concentration. Thus, the nodal region represents a zone of about 6 µm that is depleted in mitochondria. This has been reported also for axons in mice (Edgar et al., 2008).

Figure 3
Mitochondria avoid nodes of Ranvier

Noticing that astrocytes are rich in mitochondria, we wondered if astrocyte mitochondria might concentrate near nodes of Ranvier and by some unsuspected mechanism supply the node with ATP. To investigate this, we plotted the locations of all astrocytic mitochondria within 10 µm of one node (Fig 3B). Mitochondria did not cluster near the node. Analyzing 51 nodes confirmed that mitochondria are rare near a node but rise to asymptote within a few micrometers (Fig 3D). Thus astrocytes do not supply ATP to the nodes.

To understand why mitochondria do not concentrate at the node of Ranvier, we calculated from Fick’s law how rapidly sodium entering a node would redistribute. A pulse of sodium at the center of a node diffuses away from the node in both directions, and the nodal sodium concentration decreases exponentially, falling by 90% within one millisecond (Fig 3E). This suggests that sodium extrusion occurs mainly in the internodes, and that is where the mitochondria occur.

Energy capacity vs. fiber caliber

We measured the mitochondrial volume per unit length in axons of different caliber. Volume was measured as the area of a mitochondrial cross-section times the section thickness (~90nm). These measurements yielded the concentration of mitochondria in axonal cytoplasm (Fig 4A). Mitochondrial concentration rose steeply for the finest axons; then leveled off at ~1.6% for diameters greater than ~ 0.7 µm. Thus mitochondrial volume (Vm) had a quadratic relation to axonal diameter (d). Fitting a quadratic polynomial in d by minimizing squared error gave a relation of the form Vm = 0.0044 (d – 0.46) [(d – 0.46) + 4.7]. The parameters are well-constrained by the fit -- 5% change in any of them increased chi-squared by a factor of 2–3. Thus, for small diameters (~0.5 µm) the mitochondrial volume rose linearly in diameter, while for larger diameters (>0.7 µm) the relation was quadratic. The initial sub-quadratic rise probably reflects the exclusion of mitochondria from the smallest profiles, which are constrictions (Fig. 2B).

Unmyelinated axons in retina

Axons emerging from ganglion cell somas collect in the nerve fiber layer and run toward the optic disk. Over this course, they remain unmyelinated. Because their firing rates are the same as for the myelinated segments, we thought to compare their structures and energy capacities. Unmyelinated axons in retina showed a similar range of diameters as for the myelinated segments and a similar skewed distribution (Fig 5A, C). The shape of the distribution for unmyelinated segments was well described by a lognormal curve; i.e. the natural logarithm of the diameter was distributed as a Gaussian with a mean of log[0.62 ] and a standard deviation of 0.31. Here the best-fitting parameters were determined by minimizing chi-squared (assuming standard error for bins with N counts). A ~10% variation in the parameters more than doubled chi-squared. The diameter range containing 95% of the axons was 0.3 µm - 1.1 µm. Although the unmyelinated and myelinated distributions differed slightly, the samples came from different animals and probably from different eccentricities, so it is unclear whether these fine differences are reliable.

Unmyelinated axons were not separated by glial processes, as in the nerve, but instead were packed together in tight bundles (Fig 5A, B). The bundles were enveloped by processes of Muller glia. These appeared dark in electron micrographs, due to abundant glycogen (Fig 5A, B). Mitochondria were absent from Muller processes but were abundant in ganglion cell somas, dendrites, and axons (Fig 5B, D; (Kageyama and Wong-Riley, 1984). The absence of mitochondria in Muller processes of the guinea pig inner retina (Germer et al., 1998), and their abundance in retinal neurons render this brain region a ‘poster child’ for the lactate shuttle hypothesis (Pellerin and Magistretti, 1994,Poitry-Yamate et al., 1995).

In the unmyelinated segments mitochondria were 0.27 ± 0.05 µm in diameter (n=2450) – similar to the nerve. Mitchondrial concentration increased steeply with fiber caliber, reaching a plateau of 3.5% for fibers > 0.7 µm diameter and then relaxing slightly (Fig 4A). Because the mitochondrial concentration was roughly constant over a large range of diameters, the mitochondrial volume (Vm), increased quadratically with diameter (d) over this range (Fig. 4B). Fitting a quadratic polynomial in d gave a relation of the form Vm = 0.017 (d – 0.44) [(d – 0.44) + 2.5]. A 5% change in any of the parameters increased chi-squared by a factor of 2–3. Thus, the pattern of mitochondrial concentration in the unmyelinated segments resembled that of the myelinated segments, but for fibers thicker than 0.7µm, the volume fraction was two-fold greater (Fig 4A).

How the optic nerve allocates spikes

The smallest ganglion cell with the finest axon is the ‘local-edge’ type; whereas the largest ganglion cell with the thickest axon is the ‘brisk-transient’ type. The local-edge cells respond to movies of natural scenes with mean firing rates of about ~4 spikes s−1 (range: 1 −7 Hz); whereas the ON brisk-transient cells respond to the same movies with two-fold higher mean rates (Koch et al., 2006). For example, Fig. 6A shows a local-edge and a brisk-transient cell responding simultaneously to two seconds of a 20-second natural movie clip. Each cell type responds with a characteristic firing pattern, which leads to different average firing rate. If firing rates for the thinnest and thickest axons reflect a general relationship, then the distribution of firing rates should match the distribution of axon diameters. To test this, we recorded 234 neurons in three retinas during stimulation with naturalistic movies. The distribution of mean firing rates was well described by a lognormal function, i.e. the natural logarithm of the firing rate was distributed as a Gaussian with a mean of log[4.3 ± 0.8] and a standard deviation of 0.72 ± 0.7 (Fig. 6B). Here the best fitting parameters were derived by minimizing chi-squared (assuming standard error for bins with N counts), and the ranges indicate variations that doubled chi-squared.

Given that small axons fire rarely and large axons fire more, we asked if there might be a simple relationship between mean firing rate and axon diameter. We considered a relationship between axon diameter and mean firing rate, rather than peak rate, because the rapid diffusion of sodium out of the nodes (Fig. 3E) suggested that even at the highest observed firing rates, the relatively slow restoration of overall ionic concentrations in an axon is the dominant consideration. This was confirmed by compartmental modeling (see below).

The simplest possible relationship between axon diameter and mean firing rate would be linear: R = K (d – s) where R is the firing rate, d is the diameter, K is a proportionality constant and s is a minimal axon diameter. If firing rate were indeed linear with diameter, a shifted and rescaled firing rate distribution would match the axon diameter distribution. Testing this, we found that these distributions match best for K = 10.0, s = 0.46 µm (Fig. 6C). This value of s is near the finest fiber diameters in our measurements (Fig. 1E; Fig. 5C); it is the diameter below which local fluctuations in channel density apparently cause noise in spike timing (Faisal et al., 2005). A similar analysis for unmyelinated axons showed that the best match occurred for R = 13.4 (d – 0.32). A ~20% variation in the parameters in both cases increased the squared deviation between the matched distributions by a factor of ~2.

We similarly tested a possible quadratic relation between rate and diameter (R = K2 (d-s)2 + K1 (d−s)), as well a possible linear relation between the logarithms of the rate and the diameter (log(R) = log(C) + E log(d), or, equivalently, R = C dE). (We tried the latter relation because any two lognormal curves can be transformed into each other by shifting the mean and rescaling the standard deviation of the logarithm of the independent variable (x) – i.e., by a linear transformation on log(x).) The best-fitting nonlinear models (quadratic: K2 = 7.2, K1 = 0, s= 0.1; log-linear: C= 2.4 , E =0.4) gave mean-squared-deviations between the rate and diameter distributions that were similar to the linear model. However, while they matched the shape of the distributions well near their peaks, the non-linear models predicted that the largest axons in our dataset (d ~ 3.5 µm) would have mean firing rates of ~80–120 Hz, but such high rates are not seen. By contrast the linear hypothesis maps diameters of 3.5 µm to firing rates of ~30Hz which were the highest seen in our data. Consequently, we accept the hypothesis of a linear relationship between fiber diameter and firing rate. Most of the information transmitted to the brain by the eye is sent at lowest mean firing rates (Koch et al., 2006). Any monotonic relation between rate and diameter thus implies that most information is carried by the thinnest fibers.

The conclusion relating firing rates to axon diameter (Fig. 6) come from recordings in vitro. However, recordings in vivo generally yield higher spontaneous firing rates (e.g., Troy and Robson, 1992), so one might wonder whether this linear relationship holds in the intact, behaving animal. The actual firing rates in an intact animal remain unknown for two reasons: (i) the in vivo rates derive from acute preparations that are highly stressed and influenced by various anesthetics; (ii) the average response rates to naturalistic stimuli have not been studied in the acute preparation.

On the other hand, there is some evidence that in vivo rates (evoked and spontaneous) increase proportionally. First, ganglion cell detection thresholds (which depend on the ratio of evoked to spontaneous rates) are similar in vitro and in vivo. These thresholds apparently account for behavioral detection (Borghuis et al., 2009), suggesting that the evoked/spontaneous ratio might hold across recording conditions. Second, the differences between types: sluggish vs. brisk, and brisk-transient vs. brisk-sustained are seen under both conditions (Troy and Shou, 2002). Thus, the relationships between cell types, and between evoked and spontaneous firing rates, seem similar under both recording conditions. Since these relationships drive our analysis, we expect that it will extend to the intact animal. In particular, if all rates (evoked and spontaneous) for all cell types increase proportionally, the relation between rate and diameter that we find will continue to hold. In any case, as we will see, our broad conclusions will not depend specifically on a linear relationship between rate and diameter.

How the optic nerve relates energy and information

For the unmyelinated region within the retina, an axon’s energy cost per spike corresponds to the number of sodium ions needed to discharge its membrane capacitance. This rises linearly with axon diameter d (See Methods; Fig‥ 7A). The precise slope is proportional to the assumed capacitance, but for our purposes the key point is the linear increase in sodium flux, and hence, energy cost, as a function of diameter.

Figure 7
Metabolic cost of spiking

For the myelinated region within the nerve, the energy cost per spike corresponds to the number of sodium ions needed to discharge axonal capacitance at a node of Ranvier times the number of nodes. The length of a node was invariant with axon caliber, so nodal capacitance rises simply with d. However, the distance between nodes also rises with d (Hess and Young, 1949,Hess and Young, 1952). Therefore, nodal membrane capacitance per unit length should be invariant with axon caliber. Internodal membrane capacitance per unit length should also be invariant, because thicker axons have thicker sheaths (Guy et al., 1989).

We estimated sodium entry (including both the nodal and internodal regions) both by calculating the capacitance and from a multi-compartmental model tuned to operate at 370 C. Both methods gave the same result, entry of ~1200 Na+ spike−1µm−1 (Fig 7A; see Methods). The precise value depends on the assumed membrane capacitance and the conductances in the model, but for our purposes the key point is that both methods of analysis show sodium entry to be independent of diameter for myelinated axons. Importantly, both estimates assume that internodal length = axon diameter X 100 (Rushton, 1951). If this scaling relation fails for small central axons (Waxman and Bennett, 1972), our model estimates would be off. Yet, since the function of myelin is clearly to reduce capacitance and increase the space constant, one would not expect the error to be very large.

To assess the energy cost per spike in real axons we divided the mitochondrial volume for axons of each diameter (Fig 4B) by the associated firing rate (Fig 6B,C; rate-diameter match for unmyelinated axons not shown), assuming our hypothesized linear relation between rate and diameter. For unmyelinated regions, energy capacity per spike increased linearly over the range of measured axon diameters (Fig. 7B) -- as expected from theory (Fig. 7A). Therefore, under naturalistic conditions, energy capacity of unmyelinated axons apparently matches the cost of spiking. But for myelinated regions, energy capacity per spike was roughly constant over the range of measured axon diameters (Fig 7B). This was also expected from theory because the capacitance of a myelinated axon is approximately independent of diameter.

Yet, the slope deviated slightly from zero (4.89*10−04 /µm), suggesting that there must be some additional cost beyond sodium pumping. If we assumed a quadratic relation between rate and diameter (rejected above because of the poor match between the ranges of firing rates and axon diameters), the myelinated energy capacity per spike would have been constant, as expected from theory. However, in that case, the unmyelinated energy capacity per spike would have failed to rise linearly as expected. This gave an additional reason to reject the hypothesis of quadratic dependence of rate on diameter.

An axonal firing rate can be converted into an estimated information rate following a simple formula (Koch et al., 2006). For each axonal diameter, using the formula and the linear relation between diameter and firing rate (Fig 6C), we computed the information rate. Plotting these information rates against the mitochondrial volume of axons of the associated diameter, we found that information increases sublinearly with energy capacity (Fig 7C). Thus doubling the information requires more than double the energy capacity (e.g. a 2-fold change from 6 bits s−1 to 12 bits s−1 is associated to a 2.6 fold increase in mitochondrial volume). This law of diminishing returns was expected on information theoretic grounds (Balasubramanian et al., 2001; de Polavieja, 2002; Levy and Baxter, 1996; Sarpeshkar, 1998) and is here confirmed in the data. This principle was robust to substantial changes in the assumed relationship between rate an diameter – specifically, it held even for an assumed quadratic relationship between rate and diameter.

Sodium buffering does not explain the need for larger axons

Because Na+ accumulation in a small axon during a burst of spikes might delay or block conduction, we asked whether larger axon diameters are essential for buffering sodium. To test this we used the multi-compartment model that reproduced the known spike shape in mammalian myelinated axons (Fig 8A; (Koch, 1999)) and the known relationship between fiber diameter and conduction velocity (Fig 8B). We then evaluated the change in internal sodium concentration [Na+]i in the nodal compartment of the smallest diameter axon (0.2 um ). Testing the highest observed instantaneous firing rate (300 impulses s−1; (Koch et al., 2006), we extended the longest observed duration by 10-fold (30 impulses/100 ms). Under these conditions, [Na+]i in the nodal compartment steadily increased by about 10 mM (Fig 8C, gray line), in agreement with the observed [Na+]i change in Purkinje dendritic processes (Rose, 2003). Introducing Na+ diffusion into the internodal region completely eliminated this concentration increase (Fig 8C black line). These results were robust to changes in model parameters – e.g., a 25% change in assumed sodium conductance only changes the action potential shape and propagation velocity by a few percent, leaving the above conclusions unchanged. We conclude that under natural conditions increases in sodium concentration are small and, even in the finest myelinated axon, would not affect spiking. Therefore larger axons are not needed to buffer sodium.

Figure 8
Larger axons are not required for sodium buffering

DISCUSSION

Allocation of space and energy

We expected that most space and energy capacity in a central tract would be devoted to nerve fibers. However, we found that ~30% of the space is devoted to astrocytes (vs.11% in mouse cortex; (Schuz and Palm, 1989). Furthermore, 72% of the mitochondria are in astrocytes, and their volume fraction is twice that in axoplasm. Thus, although astrocytes may operate anerobically to some degree, in the nerve they appear also to maintain considerable intrinsic oxidative capacity, and one wonders what function requires this.

Each spike dumps a K+ pulse into the extracellular space. Because all fibers are firing, individual pulses are not spatially buffered and thus cause sharp, local rises in potassium. The concentration is restored in two phases: rapidly by the glial Na/K-ATPase with a low affinity for K+, and slowly by the axonal Na/K-ATPase with a high affinity for K+ (Ransom et al., 2000). Thus the role of astrocytes in rapidly reducing extracellular potassium may explain their large proportion of space and energy capacity (Fig 1).

Because the node of Ranvier is the main site of sodium influx during the action potential, we expected to find mitochondria concentrated there, similar to high mitochondrial concentrations at other sites of major cation flux, e.g., photoreceptor inner segments, basal membranes of renal tubules, and salt glands (Bloom, 1968). However, mitochondria were absent from the node (Fig 3). This seemed less surprising when we calculated that sodium entering focally at a node would diffuse rapidly to the internodes (Fig 3E). That is where the sodium-extruding Na/K-ATPase is located (McGrail and Sweadner, 1990,McGrail et al., 1991). Thus the mitochondria in myelinated axons are actually located where they are needed.

Why optic axons are mostly thin

Optic axons were rarely thinner than 0.5 µm, probably because below this diameter spontaneous channel fluctuations cause variations in spike timing sufficient to degrade the message (Faisal and Laughlin, 2007). However, starting from the smallest diameters, the distribution rises to a sharp peak near 0.7µm (Fig 1). This predominance of small diameters may occur because, although thicker axons increase firing proportionally to diameter (Fig 6C), their mitochondrial volume (hence energy capacity) increases as the diameter squared. This implies a law of diminishing returns: information rises sublinearly with energy capacity (Fig. 7C; Levy and Baxter, 1996; Sarpeshkar, 1998; Balasubramanian et al., 2001; de Polavieja, 2002; Balasubramanian and Berry, 2002; Niven et al., 2007). Thus, efficient energy use splits the information stream among many thin, low-rate carriers (Koch et al., 2006).

Why some axons are thick

The standard idea is that thick axons are required to achieve higher conduction velocity and shorter conduction times. This is true where conduction distances are long and rapid responses are essential – e.g., fast reflex loops, certain pyramidal tract fibers (Biedenbach et al., 1986), certain cortico-cortical connections in larger brains (Wang et al., 2008), and certain optic fibers in large, aquatic mammals (Mazzatenta et al., 2001). However, the need for larger axons in the optic nerve of smaller mammals does not seem to be explained by this “conduction time” hypothsis.

First, conduction distances to the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) are sufficiently short (17 mm in guinea pig) that difference in conduction times between the thinnest and thickest axons is only ~3.2 ms. This is comparable to the shortest inter-spike interval and considerably shorter than typical temporal response variability of 10–30 ms across repetitions of a stimulus (Koch et al., 2006, Meister and Berry, 1999). Thus the conduction advantage of the thick axons is unlikely to play a role in detection of visual stimuli; see (Troy and Lennie, 1987) for a similar conclusion from measurements in cat lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN). Nevertheless, one may wonder whether spikes in a fast-conducting axon could have conditioning effects on cells post-synaptic to a slow-conducting axon. This seems unlikely: (i) the large temporal variability of spike generation would render such conditioning unreliable; (ii) different ganglion cell types maintain separate pathways to their postsynaptic neurons and do not show convergence. For example, X, Y, and W ganglion cells drive X, Y, and W geniculate neurons, without cross-talk (Raczkowski et al., 1988). Overall, it seems implausible that the brain would devote significantly more space and energy for such an apparently minor reduction in conduction time.

Second, the “conduction time” hypothesis suggests that increasing conduction distance would shift the whole fiber distribution toward larger diameters. This is not supported by cross-species comparisons (mouse to man) where the optic nerve varies in length by 10-fold. The fiber distributions in all species are skewed and, far from shifting toward larger calibers with longer distances, the distributions essentially superimpose (Fig 9A). Thus, although instances certainly exist where larger axons are employed to reduce conduction time, this does not explain the larger optic axons studied here.

Fig 9
The distribution of axon diameters in conserved across species

Alternatively, if high spike rates would raise the sodium concentration in fine axons, thicker axons might be needed to increase axoplasmic volume as a buffer. However, a compartmental model showed that firing rates far exceeding the natural rates would not alter the sodium concentration even in the finest axon (Fig 8C). Thus fine axons should easily support the highest spike rates without refractoriness.

Here we propose a new hypothesis for why some axons are thick. First, recall that information represented by Poisson release of n transmitter quanta, rises as log2 of the signal/noise ratio. Doubling this ratio would require at least quadrupling n. Consequently an axon carrying twice the information rate should require more than four times the number of active zones to transfer this information at its output. Indeed, it is clear that the photoreceptors and bipolar cells with higher information rates use thicker axons and larger terminals with more active zones (Sterling, 2004; Sterling and Freed, 2007).

Ganglion cell central arbors apparently also follow this rule: (i) brisk-transient (Y) cells with higher information rates have a thicker axon with more central boutons than brisk-sustained (X) cells (Bowling and Michael, 1984; Humphrey et al., 1985a; Humphrey et al., 1985b; Roe et al., 1989). Moreover, Y boutons are larger and contain more active zones (Hamos et al., 1985). Sluggish (W) cells with the lowest information rates have the thinnest axons and contact W cells in the LGN via fewer synapses than used by Y or X cells (Raczkowski et al., 1988). Given these facts, we propose that axon caliber in the optic nerve increases with mean spike rate, not to achieve any intrinsic electrophysiological advantage, but rather to support larger terminal arbors and the extra active zones needed to transfer information synaptically at higher rates. Cells with higher information rates may also supply more central nuclei, and this would further increase their needs for axonal transport (Tamamaki et al., 1995).

This hypothesis might also fit other types of central neuron. For example, pyramidal neurons with higher firing rates (thus higher information rates) have faster conducting, i.e., thicker, axons (Armstrong and Drew, 1984). And Drosophila neurons with more voluminous terminal arbors express more active zones (Egger et al., 1997).

These points suggest a broad conclusion: an axon’s cross-sectional area rises proportionally with the number of active zones at its output. This expanded cross-section accommodates additional microtubules to serve the added burden of axonal transport (Sterling and Freed, 2007). Microtubules in fine axons exhibit fixed spacing (Peters et al., 1991, Hsu et al., 1998), so increasing their number requires expansion of the axonal cross-section. Thicker axons also contain neurofilaments as cytoskeletal elements (Peters et al., 1991) that contribute to the expansion. Thus the increase in axon volume with spike rate may be fundamentally set by the need to supply larger central arbors that transfer higher information rates.

What sets the most common axon diameter?

The optic nerve’s most common axon diameter is 0.7µm. Perhaps this value is set to reduce the cost of communication by ganglion cells. Suppose, for example, that optic axons with their terminal arbors maximize their information per unit energy. Assume that a neuron’s total cost is proportional to

E=Vm+C
(3)

where Vm is the mitochondrial volume, and C (in units of mitochondrial volume per unit axonal length, µm3) reflects basal costs that are independent of firing rate. Then Information/E can be computed numerically from Fig. 7C. The result is that bits per cost is maximized at a diameter of 0.7 µm -- if C=0.0015 µm3, which is comparable to the mitochondrial volumes in very fine axons (diameter ~0.5 µm; Fig. 9B). Thus, optimizing neuronal communication with respect to energy use might determine the typical axonal diameter. The existence of such an optimum simply requires a law of diminishing returns relating information to energy (Fig. 7C); it does not require the linear relation between rate and diameter for which we found evidence. This optimization includes one parameter, a basal cost C, whose measurement would allow one to evaluate this models.

Supplementary Material

Supp1

Acknowledgements

We thank Jian Li for electron microscopy and Sharron Fina for help with the figures. In addition we are grateful to Michael Sikora for programming, Quan Wen for providing software, and Michael Berry for the use of his multi-electrode recording system. We also benefited from discussions with Jurgen Fohlmeister and Robert Smith and from comments on the manuscript by Steven Scherer, Gary Matthews, and Simon Laughlin. Supported by NIH grant EY 08124, NSF Grant IBN-0344678 and NIH grant R01 EY03014. Vijay Balasubramanian was supported in part as the Helen and Martin Chooljian member of the Institute for Advanced Study.

Reference List

  • Armstrong DM, Drew T. Discharges of pyramidal tract and other motor cortical neurones during locomotion in the cat. J Physiol. 1984;346:471–495. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Attwell D, Laughlin S. An energy budget for signaling in the grey matter of the brain. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism. 2001;21:1133–1145. [PubMed]
  • Balasubramanian V, Berry MJ. A test of metabolically efficient coding in the retina. Network. 2002;13:531–552. [PubMed]
  • Balasubramanian V, Kimber D, Berry MJ. Metabolically efficient information processing. Neural Comp. 2001;13:799–815. [PubMed]
  • Biedenbach MA, DeVito JL, Brown AC. Pyramidal tract of the cat: axon size and morphology. Exp Brain Res. 1986;61:303–310. [PubMed]
  • Bloom W. A textbook of histology. Philadelphia PA: W.B.Saunders Co; 1968.
  • Borghuis BG, Ratliff CP, Smith RG, Sterling P, Balasubramanian V. Design of a neuronal array. J Neurosci. 2009;28:3178–3189. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Bowling DB, Michael CR. Terminal patterns of single, physiologically characterized optic tract fibers in the cat's lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurosci. 1984;4:198–216. [PubMed]
  • de Polavieja GG. Errors drive the evolution of biological signalling to costly codes. J Theor Biol. 2002;214:657–664. [PubMed]
  • Eccles JC, Kostyuk PG, Schmidt RF. Central pathways responsible for depolarization of primary afferent fibres. J Physiol. 1962;161:237–257. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Edgar JM, McCulloch MC, Thomson CE, Griffiths IR. Distribution of mitochondria along small diameter myelinated central nervous system axons. J Neurosci Res. 2008;86:2250–2257. [PubMed]
  • Egger MD, Nowakowski RS, Peng B, Wyman RJ. Patterns of connectivity in a Drosophila nerve. J Comp Neurol. 1997;387:63–72. [PubMed]
  • Faisal AA, Laughlin SB. Stochastic simulations on the reliability of action potential propagation in thin axons. PLoS. 2007;3:783–795. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Faisal AA, White JA, Laughlin SB. Ion-channel noise places limits on the miniaturization of the brain's wiring. Curr Biol. 2005;15:1143–1149. [PubMed]
  • Fohlmeister JF, Miller RF. Impulse encoding mechanisms of ganglion cells in the tiger salamander retina. J Neurophysiol. 1997a;78:1935–1947. [PubMed]
  • Fohlmeister JF, Miller RF. Mechanisms by which cell geometry controls repetitive impulse firing in retinal ganglion cells. J Neurophysiol. 1997b;78:1948–1964. [PubMed]
  • Fohlmeister JF. A nerve model of greatly increased energy-efficient and encoding flexibility over the Hodgkin-Huxley model. Submitted to Brain Research. 2009 [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Friede RI, Benda M, Dewitz A, Stoll P. Relations between axon length and axon clier. "Is maximum conduction velocity the factor controlling the evolution of nerve structure"? Journal of Neurological Sciences. 1984;63:369–380. [PubMed]
  • Friede RI, Beuche W. A new approach toward analyzing peripheral nerve fiber populations: I. Variance in sheath thickness corresponds to different geometric proportions of the internodes. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology. 1985;44:60–72. [PubMed]
  • Germer A, Biederman B, Wolburg H, Schuck J, Grosche J, Kuhrt H, Reichelt W, Schousboe A, Paasche G, Mack AF, Reichenbach A. Distribution of mitochondria within Muller cells. I. Correlation with retinal vascularization in different mammlian species. J Neurocytol. 1998;27:329–345. [PubMed]
  • Guy J, Ellis EA, Kelley K, Hope GM. Spectra of G ratio, myelin sheath thickness, and axon and fiber diameter in the guinea pig optic nerve. J Comp Neurol. 1989;287:446–454. [PubMed]
  • Hackenbrock CR. Chemical and physical fixation ofisolated mitochondria in low-energy and high-energy states. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1968a;61:598–605. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hackenbrock CR. Ultrastructural bases for metabolically linked mechanical activity in itochondria. II. electron transport-linked ultrastructural transformations in mitochondria. J Cell Biol. 1968b;37:345–369. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hamos JE, van Horn SC, Raczkowski D, Uhlrich DJ, Sherman SM. Synaptic connectivity of a local circuit neruone in lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat. Nature. 1985;317:618–621. [PubMed]
  • Hess A, Young JZ. Nodes of Ranvier in the central nervous system. J Physiol. 1949;108 Sup:52. [PubMed]
  • Hess A, Young JZ. The nodes of Ranvier. Proc R Soc Lond B. 1952;140:301–320. [PubMed]
  • Hines ML, Carnevale NT. The NEURON simulation environment. Neural Comp. 1997;9:1179–1209. [PubMed]
  • Hodgkin A. The optimum density of sodium channels in an unmyelinated nerve. Philos Trans R Soc Lond (Biol) 1975;270:297–300. [PubMed]
  • Hsu SC, Hazuka CD, Roth R, Foletti DL, Heuser J, Scheller RH. Subunit composition, protein interactions, and structures of the mammalian brain sec 6/8 complex and septin filaments. Neuron. 1998;20:1111–1122. [PubMed]
  • Humphrey AL, Sur M, Uhlrich DJ. Termination patterns of individual X- and Y-cell axons in the visual cortex of the cat: projections to area 18, to the 17/18 border region, and to both areas. J Comp Neurol. 1985a;233:190–212. [PubMed]
  • Humphrey AL, Sur M, Uhlrich DJ, Sherman SM. Projection patterns of individual X- and Y-cell axons from the lateral geniculate nucleus to cortical area 17 in the cat. J Comp Neurol. 1985b;233:159–189. [PubMed]
  • Hunt CC. Relation of function to diameter in afferent fiers of muscle nerves. J Gen Physiol. 1954;38:117–131. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hursh JB. Conduction velocity and diameter of nerve fibers. Am J Physiol. 1939;127:131–139.
  • Kageyama GH, Wong-Riley MT. The histochemical localization of cytochrome oxidase in the retina and lateral geniculate nucleus of the ferret, cat, and monkey, with particular reference to retinal mosaics and ON/OFF-center visual channels. J Neurosci. 1984;4:2445–2459. [PubMed]
  • Kao Y-H, Sterling P. Displaced GAD65 amacrine cells of the guinea pig retina are morphologically diverse. Vis Neurosci. 2006 [PubMed]
  • Koch C. Biophysics of computation: information processing in single neurons. New York NY: Oxford University Press; 1999.
  • Koch K, McLean J, Berry M, Sterling P, Balasubramanian V, Freed MA. Efficiency of information transmission by retinal ganglion cells. Curr Biol. 2004;14:1523–1530. [PubMed]
  • Koch K, McLean J, Segev R, Freed MA, Berry MJII, Balasubramanian V, Sterling P. How much the eye tells the brain. Curr Biol. 2006;16:1428–1434. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Laughlin SB, Sejnowski TJ. Communication in neuronal networks. Science. 2003;301:1870–1873. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Lennie P. The cost of cortical computation. Curr Biol. 2003;13:493–497. [PubMed]
  • Levy WB, Baxter RA. Energy efficient neural codes. Neural Comp. 1996;8:531–543. [PubMed]
  • Mazzatenta A, Caleo M, Baldaccini NE, Maffei L. A comparative morphometric analysis of the optic nerve in two cetacean species, the striped dolphin (Stenella coeruleoalba) and fin whale (Balaenoptera physalus) Vis Neurosci. 2001;18:319–325. [PubMed]
  • McGrail KM, Phillips JM, Sweadner KJ. Immunofluorescent localization of three Na, K-ATPase isozymes in the rat central nervous system: both neurons and glia can express more than one Na,K-ATPase. J Neurosci. 1991;11:381–391. [PubMed]
  • McGrail KM, Sweadner KJ. Complex expression patterns for Na+, K+,-ATPase isoforms in retina and optic nerve. Eur J Neurosci. 1990;2:170–176. [PubMed]
  • Meister M, Berry MJ. The neural code of the retina. Neuron. 1999;22:435–450. [PubMed]
  • Niven JE, Anderson JC, Laughlin SB. Fly photoreceptors demonstrate energy-information trade-offs in neural coding. PLoS. 2007;5:e116. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Pellerin L, Magistretti PJ. Glutamate uptake into astrocytes stimulates aerobic glycolysis: a mechanism coupling neuronal activity to glucose utilization. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1994;91:10625–10629. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Peters A, Palay SL, Webster H. The fine structure of the nervous system: neurons and their supporting cells. New York NY: Oxford University Press; 1991.
  • Poitry-Yamate C, Poitry S, Tsacopoulous M. Lactate released by Müller glial cells is metabolized by photoreceptors from mammalian retina. J Neurosci. 1995;15:5179–5191. [PubMed]
  • Raczkowski D, Hamos JE, Sherman SM. Synaptic circuitry of physiologically identified W-cells in the cat's dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurosci. 1988;8:31–48. [PubMed]
  • Ransom CB, Ransom BR, Sontheimer H. Activity-dependent extracellular K+ accumulation in rat optic nerve: the role of glial and axonal Na+ pumps. J Physiol. 2000;522:427–442. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Roe AW, Garraghty PE, Sur M. Terminal arbors of single ON-center and OFF-center X and Y retrinal ganglion cell axons within the ferret's lateral geniculate nucleus. J Comp Neurol. 1989;288:208–242. [PubMed]
  • Rose CR. High-resolution Na+ imaging in dendrites and spines. Pflügers Arch. 2003;446:317–321. [PubMed]
  • Rushton WA. A theory of the effects of fibre size in medullated nerve. J Physiol. 1951;115:101–122. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Sarpeshkar R. Analog versus digital: extrapolating from electronics to neurobiology. Neural Comp. 1998;10:1601–1638. [PubMed]
  • Scaduto RC, Grotyohann LW. Measurement of mitochondrial membrane potential using fluorescent rhodamine derivatives. Biophys J. 1999;76:469–477. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Scherer SS. Nodes, paranodes, and incisures: from form to function. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1999;883:131–142. [PubMed]
  • Schuz A, Palm G. Density of neurons and synapses in the cerebral cortex of the mouse. J Comp Neurol. 1989;286:442–455. [PubMed]
  • Segev R, Goodhouse J, Puchalla J, Berry MJ. Recording spikes from a large fraction of the ganglion cells in a retinal patch. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:1154–1161. [PubMed]
  • Sterling P. How retinal circuits optimize the transfer of visual information. In: Chalupa LM, Werner JS, editors. The Visual Neurosciences. Cambridge MA: MIT Press; 2004. pp. 243–268.
  • Sterling P, Freed MA. How robust is a neural circuit? Vis Neurosci. 2007 [PubMed]
  • Tamamaki N, Uhlrich DJ, Sherman SM. Morphology of physiologically identified retinal X and Y axons in the cat's thalamus and midbrain as revealed by intraaxonal injection of biocytin. J Comp Neurol. 1995;354:583–607. [PubMed]
  • Troy JB, Lennie P. Detection latencies of X and Y type cells of the cat's dorsal lateral geniculte nucleus. Exp Brain Res. 1987;65:703–706. [PubMed]
  • Troy JB, Robson JG. Steady discharges of X and Y retinal ganglion cells of cat under photopic illuminance. Vis Neurosci. 1992;9:535–553. [PubMed]
  • Troy JB, Shou T. The receptive fields of cat retinal ganglion cells in physiological and pathological states: where we are after half a century of research. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2002;21:263–302. [PubMed]
  • Tsukamoto Y, Masarachia P, Schein SJ, Sterling P. Gap junctions between the pedicles of macaque foveal cones. Vision Res. 1992;32:1809–1815. [PubMed]
  • Wang SS-H, Shultz JR, Burish MJ, Harrison KH, Hof PR, Towns LC, Wagers MW, Wyatt KD. Functional trade-offs in white matter axonal scaling. J Neurosci. 2008;28:4047–4056. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Waxman SG, Bennett MV. Relative conduction velocities of small myeliinated and non-myelinated fibres in the central nervous system. Nature New Biology. 1972;238:217–219. [PubMed]
  • Weibel ER. Symmorphosis. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press; 2000.
  • Wen Q, Chklovskii DB. Segregation of the brain into gray and white matter: a design minimizing conduction delays. PLoS Computational Biology. 2005;1:0617–0630. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Williams RW, Chalupa LM. An analysis of axon caliber within the optic nerve of the cat: evidence of size groupings and regional organization. J Neurosci. 1983;3:1554–1564. [PubMed]
PubReader format: click here to try

Formats:

Related citations in PubMed

See reviews...See all...

Cited by other articles in PMC

See all...

Links

Recent Activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...