• We are sorry, but NCBI web applications do not support your browser and may not function properly. More information
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
JAMA. Author manuscript; available in PMC Jul 27, 2010.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2910591
NIHMSID: NIHMS24850

Long-term Effects of Cognitive Training on Everyday Functional Outcomes in Older Adults

Abstract

Context

Cognitive training has been shown to improve cognitive abilities in older adults but the effects of cognitive training on everyday function have not been demonstrated.

Objective

To determine the effects of cognitive training on daily function and durability of training on cognitive abilities.

Design, Setting, and Participants

Five-year follow-up of a randomized controlled single-blind trial with 4 treatment groups. A volunteer sample of 2832 persons (mean age, 73.6 years; 26% black), living independently in 6 US cities, was recruited from senior housing, community centers, and hospitals and clinics. The study was conducted between April 1998 and December 2004. Five-year follow-up was completed in 67% of the sample.

Interventions

Ten-session training for memory (verbal episodic memory), reasoning (inductive reasoning), or speed of processing (visual search and identification); 4-session booster training at 11 and 35 months after training in a random sample of those who completed training.

Main Outcome Measures

Self-reported and performance-based measures of daily function and cognitive abilities.

Results

The reasoning group reported significantly less difficulty in the instrumental activities of daily living (IADL) than the control group (effect size, 0.29; 99% confidence interval [CI], 0.03-0.55). Neither speed of processing training (effect size, 0.26; 99% CI, −0.002 to 0.51) nor memory training (effect size, 0.20; 99% CI, −0.06 to 0.46) had a significant effect on IADL. The booster training for the speed of processing group, but not for the other 2 groups, showed a significant effect on the performance-based functional measure of everyday speed of processing (effect size, 0.30; 99% CI, 0.08-0.52). No booster effects were seen for any of the groups for everyday problem-solving or self-reported difficulty in IADL. Each intervention maintained effects on its specific targeted cognitive ability through 5 years (memory: effect size, 0.23 [99% CI, 0.11-0.35]; reasoning: effect size, 0.26 [99% CI, 0.17-0.35]; speed of processing: effect size, 0.76 [99% CI, 0.62-0.90]). Booster training produced additional improvement with the reasoning intervention for reasoning performance (effect size, 0.28; 99% CI, 0.12-0.43) and the speed of processing intervention for speed of processing performance (effect size, 0.85; 99% CI, 0.61-1.09).

Conclusions

Reasoning training resulted in less functional decline in self-reported IADL. Compared with the control group, cognitive training resulted in improved cognitive abilities specific to the abilities trained that continued 5 years after the initiation of the intervention.

Trial Registration

clinicaltrials.gov Identifier: NCT00298558

Decline in cognitive abilities has been shown to lead to an increased risk of difficulty in performing instrumental activities of daily living (IADL).1-5 However, whether interventions to maintain or enhance cognitive abilities in older adults will prevent or delay these functional difficulties is unclear. Prior interventions with older adults have targeted those with cognitive deficits or functional disabilities and have focused on remediation rather than prevention.6,7 Prior studies have shown that cognitive interventions can improve cognitive abilities in normal elders but have not included functional outcome measures and have been limited by small, homogeneous samples and lack of randomization.8-11

The Advanced Cognitive Training for Independent and Vital Elderly (ACTIVE) study is the first multicenter, randomized controlled trial to examine the long-term outcomes of cognitive interventions on the daily functioning of older individuals living independently.12 Previously reported data from the ACTIVE study showed that each of 3 cognitive interventions improved the cognitive ability it targeted and these improvements were maintained through the 2 years of follow-up.13 This article addresses the long-term effects of cognitive training on the maintenance of self-reported IADL.

Figure 1 presents the conceptual model for the ACTIVE trial. Four hypotheses were derived from this model. First, cognitive training would affect the cognitive ability targeted by that training and these effects would be maintained over time. Second, maintained improvements in cognitive ability would have a positive transfer effect on everyday function. Based on prior research,14,15 we expected to first see this transfer of training effects to affect IADL functioning. Third, this training transfer from cognitive abilities to IADL function would occur in all 3 training groups because each of the 3 cognitive abilities has been associated with IADL functioning.9,16 While the broadest transfer of training effects was expected to improve IADL functioning, we also expected to see other training-specific effects (ie, transfer of improvement in reasoning and memory to improvements in everyday problem solving or transfer of improvement in speed of processing to improvements in everyday speed of processing tasks). Finally, because of the functional independence of participants at baseline, it was hypothesized that observations of training effects on IADL functioning would be delayed until the control group began to experience significant functional decline. This was observed at the 5-year follow-up and therefore the hypothesized delayed outcome of maintenance of IADL function could be tested.

Figure 1
Conceptual Model of ACTIVE Trial

METHODS

Participants

The sample consisted of older adults living independently with good functional and cognitive status who were recruited from senior housing, community centers, and hospitals and clinics in Birmingham, Ala; Detroit, Mich; Boston, Mass; Baltimore, Md; Indianapolis, Ind; and State College, Pa. Participants were enrolled in the study between March 1998 and October 1999.12 Persons were excluded if they were younger than 65 years; had substantial functional impairment (≥2 ADL disabilities) or cognitive decline (Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE] score ≤22)17; self-reported diagnosis of Alzheimer disease; medical conditions associated with imminent functional decline or death; severe losses in vision, hearing, or communicative ability that would interfere with study participation; recent cognitive training; or were unavailable during the study period. Because previous studies of cognitive training had been conducted in white older adults, recruitment of other race and ethnic groups was emphasized. Race was self-reported as white, black, Asian, Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, American Indian/Alaskan Native, or biracial. Participants indicated if they were Hispanic or Latino.

Study Design

ACTIVE was a randomized controlled, Single-blind trial, using a 4-group design, including 3 treatment groups and a control group (Figure 2). Participants were randomized to a group by the data coordinating center using a computer randomization program. Assessments were conducted at baseline, following the intervention, and annually at 1, 2, 3, and 5 years. Assessors were blinded to treatment assignment. Exposure to social contact was equated in the 3 intervention groups. The control group had no contact; a placebo social contact control was not included because prior intervention studies indicated no differences between a social contact control and a “no contact” control in either cognitive or functional improvement.18,19 Prior ACTIVE results13 showed that cognitive effects were specific to each intervention. The study protocol was approved by the institutional review boards at all sites and the trial was monitored by a data and safety monitoring board. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Figure 2
Flow of Individuals in ACTIVE Trial

Interventions

Each of the 3 training interventions was designed to narrowly target a specific cognitive ability—memory, reasoning, or speed of processing—and included no overlap with the functional outcomes in this study.12,13 Memory training involved teaching mnemonic strategies (organization, visualization, association) for remembering verbal material (eg, word lists, texts).8,20 Reasoning training involved teaching strategies for finding the pattern in a letter or word series (eg, a c e g i … ) and identifying the next item in the series.9,21 Speed of processing training involved visual search and divided attention (identifying an object on a computer screen at increasingly brief exposures followed by dividing attention between 2 search tasks).16,22,23 Each training intervention was 10 sessions. Only 10% of the 60- to 75-minute training sessions focused on applying these strategies to solving everyday problems (eg, mnemonic strategies to remember a grocery list; reasoning strategies to understand the pattern in a bus schedule).

Booster training was conducted at 11 and 35 months after the initial training sessions and involved four 75-minute sessions. The goal of the booster sessions was to maintain the improvement in cognitive ability and the content of these sessions was similar to the training sessions, again focusing on strategies related to the cognitive abilities not on functional outcomes. Participants who completed the initial training were eligible for booster training.12,13 A subsample of eligible participants was selected for booster training using a random number computer program. Given this selection contingency, participants who were selected and agreed to booster training were younger (P= .007) and had higher baseline cognitive function as evidenced by MMSE scores (P= .008) compared with participants who were eligible and not assigned to booster training and participants who were not eligible for booster training. Sixty percent of selected participants completed booster training at year 1 and year 3; 19% completed year 1 booster only, 6% completed year 3 booster only; and 15% did not complete any booster training. The last booster sessions were completed 2 years prior to the 5-year follow-up.

Outcome Measures

Cognitive outcomes were used to assess cognitive training effects and functional outcomes were used to assess the impact of improved cognitive abilities on instrumental functioning (see Jobe et al12 for a detailed description).

Cognitive outcomes assessed the effects of each intervention on the cognitive ability trained. Memory training outcomes involved 3 measures of verbal memory ability: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test, and the Rivermead Behavioral Paragraph Recall test.24-26 Reasoning training outcomes involved 3 reasoning ability measures: letter series, letter sets, and word series.27-29 Speed of processing training outcomes involved 3 useful field of view subscales.30-32

Functional outcomes assessed whether the cognitive interventions had an effect on daily function. Everyday functioning represented the participant’S selfratings of difficulty (IADL difficulty from the Minimum Data Set–Home Care33 and ranged from “independent” to “total dependence” on a 6-point scale) in completing cognitively demanding tasks involved in meal preparation, house-work, finances, health maintenance, telephone use, and shopping. Two performance-based categories of daily function were also assessed. Everyday problem solving34 assessed ability to reason and comprehend information in common everyday tasks (eg, identifying information in medication labels). Performance was measured with printed materials (eg, yellow pages, using the Everyday Problems Test34) and behavioral simulations (eg, making change, using the Observed Tasks of Daily Living35). These measures were hypothesized to be most closely related to reasoning and memory abilities due to their task demands. Everyday speed of processing assessed participants’ speed in interacting with realworld stimuli (eg, looking up a telephone number, using the Timed IADL Test36), and the ability to react quickly to 1 of 4 road signs (Complex Reaction Time Test22), which was hypothesized to be the most closely related to speed of processing.

Because most outcomes were assessed by multiple measures, composite scores were formed by data reduction. Each measure was standardized to its baseline values and then an average of the equally weighted standardized scores was calculated. These composite scores permitted inferences about training effects at the level of outcome rather than at the level of a single test. The composite scores were consequently more reliable than Single measures and also reduced the number of outcome analyses needed, reducing the overall type I error rate.

Analysis

To evaluate the effects of ACTIVE training, a repeated-measures, mixed-effects model37 was used, including all assigned participants (for both initial training and booster training) consistent with the intention-to-treat principle. The dependent variables were the cognitive and functional outcomes at baseline and at 5-year follow-up. The scores for each test were transformed using the Blom transformation.38,39 This transformation standardized the components in each domain to have equal weight and reduced skewness in the measures.

Independent variables were restricted to basic design features: fixed effects for treatment group, time, assignment to booster training, field site, and replicate within a site. Interaction terms were chosen for importance and interpretability: time × training, representing the net effect of the trial; booster × training, representing nonspecific effects of attending booster training regardless of content; time × booster × training, representing training-specific effects of each booster intervention; and replicate within a site, representing variation between and within field sites. Because there were no Significant baseline differences in cognition and function across groups, these interaction terms capture the divergence across groups that can be attributed to the training interventions. Because participants who were assigned to booster training were younger and had higher baseline cognitive function, the model comparing booster and nonbooster participants was then repeated controlling for baseline age and MMSE score. The models were fitted to the available data, ignoring missing data. To determine if selective attrition influenced the trial results, missing data were imputed using multiple imputation procedures.40 The analysis was repeated and there were no differences in the results; therefore, the data presented do not include imputed values. All data analysis was conducted by statisticians at the data coordinating center using SAS statistical software version 9.2 (SAS Institute Inc, Cary, NC).

Hypotheses were tested by comparing the net effect at year 5 in each treatment group with the net effect in the control group. The net effect of training at year 5 was defined as mean improvement from baseline to year 5 in the intervention group minus the mean improvement from baseline to year 5 in the control group divided by the intrasubject SD of the Blorn-transformed score. Similarly, the effect of each cognitive ability–specific booster training was defined as mean improvement from baseline to year 5 in the booster-trained group minus the mean improvement from baseline to year 5 in the nonbooster-trained group divided by the intrasubject SD of the score. Participants in the nonbooster group were those who received initial training but no booster training. Results are expressed as effect sizes (ie, difference in means divided by intrasubject SD) to allow direct comparison of different outcomes.

Power calculations were based on the assumptions of 6 Bonferroni-corrected 2-sided comparisons with an overall α error of .05 (.008 for each comparison), a correlation of 0.7 between baseline and follow-up (based on pilot data), and completion rates of 80% at 2 years and 65% at 5 years. Following the methods of Cohen,41 the sample size of 2802 has 95% power to detect an effect size of 0.20. Based on the same assumptions, there is 90% power to detect booster training effects in the comparison between the subgroup receiving booster training in a training condition and the control group. In reporting statistically significant training effects, we used 99% confidence intervals (CIs; p= .008) to adjust for multiple comparisons.

RESULTS

Of 5000 individuals contacted for participation, 2832 persons were eligible, 905 (18.1%) were ineligible, and 1263 (25.3%) refused to participate (Figure 2). Compared with those who refused, those who participated were less likely to be women (76% vs 79%), were younger (mean age, 74 vs 75 years), more likely to be white (73% vs 60%), married (36% vs 27%), and better educated (mean of l3.5 vs 12.3 years). Their MMSE score was higher (mean 27.3 vs 26.8) and they were less likely to receive help with bathing (<1% needed help vs 2%). For both groups, 1% or fewer needed help with dressing and personal hygiene. Finally, those participating were less likely to have heart disease (11% vs 14%) and diabetes (13% vs 17%) but equally likely to have arthritis (57% vs 58%), stroke (0% in both groups), and cancer (6% in both groups).

Thirty individuals were randomized inappropriately in violation of the protocol and excluded from the analysis. Therefore, the analytic sample consists of 2802 randomized participants. Baseline characteristics for each of the 4 study groups appear in Table 1. Eighty-nine percent of participants completed treatment (>8/10 training sessions). Participants completing training were younger, had more education, and had higher baseline scores for the MMSE and cognitive function tests. Sixty-seven percent of the sample was retained 5 years after training despite the advanced age of the cohort. Selectivity of attrition was examined by multiple logistic regression modeling, considering sex, education, baseline age, MMSE score, and health status (number of health conditions; Short-Form 36) as well as intervention group and booster (vs nonbooster) status as predictors of retention at 5 years. Participants who were older, male, had less education and more health problems, and had lower cognitive function (indicated by lower baseline scores on the MMSE and the memory and reasoning tests) were less likely to be retained at 5 years. Importantly, there were no Significant interactions between treatment group assignment and these covariates; that is, biases in retention are over the entire sample and not related to a particular treatment group, and therefore do not affect the between-group comparisons of intervention effects.

Table 1
Baseline Characteristics

Training Effects on Cognitive Abilities

Each intervention produced immediate improvement in the cognitive ability trained 13 that was retained across 5 years (Table 2). Similarly, when controlling for baseline age and cognitive function, booster training for the reasoning and speed of processing groups produced significantly better performance (net of initial training effect) on their targeted cognitive outcomes13 that remained Significant at 5 years (Table 3).

Table 2
Effect of Training on Cognitive Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5
Table 3
Effect of Booster Training on Cognitive Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5*

Training Effects on Daily Functioning

Self-Reported IADL Difficulty. At year 5, participants in all 3 intervention groups reported less difficulty compared with the control group in performing IADL (Table 4). However, this effect was Significant only for the reasoning group, which compared with the control group had an effect size of 0.29 (99% CI, 0.03-0.55) for difficulty in performing IADL. Neither speed of processing training (effect size, 0.26; 99% Cl, −0.002 to 0.51) nor memory training (effect size, 0.20; 99% CI, −0.06 to 0.46) had a Significant effect on IADL. Figure 3 presents the standardized (Blom-transformed) mean IADL difficulty scores. Group mean IADL difficulty ratings improved through the first 2 years of the study (baseline through year 2). The decline in function for all groups is first evident between years 2 and 3. From years 3 to 5, the decline is dramatically accelerated for the control group and to a lesser extent for the 3 treatment groups.

Figure 3
Training Effects on Everyday Function by Self-reported Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL) Difficulty Scores
Table 4
Effect of Training on Functional Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5

Performance-Based IADL Measures

Training had no general effect on the performance-based measures of everyday problem solving or everyday speed of processing (Table 4). However, after controlling for baseline age and cognitive function, the effect size for those participants in the speed of processing training group assigned to receive additional booster training was 0.30 (99% CI, 0.08-0.52) better for performance on everyday speed of processing compared with participants in this group not assigned to booster training (Table 5).

Table 5
Effect of Booster Training on Functional Outcomes From Baseline to Year 5*

COMMENT

The ACTIVE study is the first large-scale, randomized trial to show that cognitive training improves cognitive function in well-functioning older adults and that this improvement lasts up to 5 years from the beginning of the intervention. In addition, this is the first trial to provide limited evidence that improvements in cognitive function can have a positive effect on daily function. Participants who received cognitive training reported less difficulty with IADLs 5 years after training compared with those in the control group. Participants in the 3 training groups reported an IADL decline of at least 0.20 SD less than the participants in the control group. The effect size reached statistical Significance only for the reasoning training group but the effect sizes seen for memory and speed of processing training were similar to that for reasoning. We consider the relative comparability of the training effects across the 3 interventions to support the clinical meaningfulness of these results. Self-report of functioning repeatedly has been shown to predict loss of independence, increased use of health services, and monality.42 Participants in the intervention groups reported similarly lower declines in function compared with participants in the control group.

The finding of less decline in self-reported everyday function is conceptually complex. Training effects could reflect real benefits of intervention because each of the 3 trained cognitive abilities has been associated with everyday functioning.22,43,44 It was not possible to blind participants to their treatment assignment (that is, participants knew whether they were receiving training). Thus, the effects of training on self-reported IADL function could also reflect personal beliefs and selfperception related to knowing whether one has received training. While self-report bias or overestimation of functional abilities has been reported in individuals with cognitive impairment,45,46 it is unlikely in this study because ACTIVE participants were screened for dementia and showed improved cognitive function in their trained ability over the study period compared with the control group. These results suggest that for self-reported IADls, a placebo control group that would facilitate blinding and additional analyses of the mechanisms of training-related improvement (eg, whether trainingrelated improvements in self-efficacy mediated the self-rated functional improvements) will be important future directions for this kind of research.

There is growing consensus that assessment of daily function requires both self-report and performance-based measures.47 The ACTIVE study is one of the few large-scale intervention studies to include both self-report and performance-based outcome measures of function to assess the breadth of intervention effects. Consistent with the study’S conceptual model, we hypothesized that certain interventions would specifically affect 1 or more functional outcomes. This was observed for the performance-based functional measures. When controlling for baseline age and cognitive function, booster training for speed of processing showed a Significant effect on the performance-based everyday speed of processing measure. This specific effect is consistent with prior research that indicated everyday speed of processing was more closely related to speed of processing9,13 than to reasoning or memory. The fact that this effect occurred only in participants who received booster training, and not in those who received only initial training, may reflect a need for larger doses of training before effects can be observed in this more cognitively demanding outcome.

Across all outcomes, evidence for transfer of the effects of cognitive training to function was modest and was not observed until the 5-year follow-up. There are 2 possible explanations for these delayed outcomes. First, prior research has suggested a temporal lag between onset of cognitive decline and subsequent impact on daily function,2-4 perhaps because of resulting difficulty in adapting to emerging physical limitations that affect tasks of daily living.48,49 However, if cognitive ability is maintained or enhanced by training, then this could result in a gradual emergence of adaptive, compensatory strategies for dealing with physical limirations.1 Second, delayed intervention effects on function may be attributable to the advantaged nature of the ACTIVE sample, and particularly the subset who received booster training. To select older adults most likely to benefit from cognitive training, persons with suspected cognitive and ADL decline were excluded from enrollment. This may have delayed the onset of functional disabilities in the control group until the 5-year follow-up. Only after the onset of decline in the control group could the positive training effects on function be observed in the intervention groups.

We are not aware of intervention trials with volunteer samples of older adults that include 5-year follow-up for direct comparison of retention rates. The 67% retention rate at 5 years is consistent with other longitudinal community-based studies resembling ACTIVE in terms of sample age and ethnicity and frequency and duration of study contact (eg, 70.6% in a 4-year study of blacks and whites in Chicago50 ; 61.6% in a 5-year study of blacks in Indianapolis51 : and 59.6% in a 7-year study of Hispanics, whites, and blacks in New York City52). However, because these studies were not intervention trials, they did not have the same level of respondent burden as ACTIVE. Therefore, while the direct comparability of retention rates to other intervention trials cannot be made, the rates of these observational studies suggests that the retention rate in ACTIVE is quite acceptable given the burdensome nature of the study protocol.

In conclusion, declines in cognitive abilities have been shown to lead to increased risk of functional disabilities that are primary risk factors for loss of independence. The 5-year results of the ACTIVE study provide limited evidence that cognitive interventions can reduce age-related decline in self-reported IADls that are the precursors of dependence in basic ADLs associated with increased use of hospital, outpatient, home health, and nursing home services.53,54 and health care expenditures.55 However, given the lag in the relationship between cognitive decline and functional deficits, the full extent of the interventional effects on daily function would take longer than 5 years to observe in a population that was highly functioning at enrollment. We consider these results promising and support future research to examine if these and other cognitive interventions can prevent or delay functional disability in an aging population.

Acknowledgment

We thank Robin Barr, PhD, of the National Institute on Aging for his authorship of the request for applications and his continuing support and helpful comments throughout the conduct of the project Dr Barr was not compensated for this assistance.

Funding/Support: ACTIVE is supported by grants from the National Institute on Aging and the National Institute of Nursing Researchto Hebrew Senior life (U01 NR04507), Indiana University School of Medicine (U01 NR04508), Johns Hopkins University (U01AG14260), New England Research Institutes (U01 AG14282), Pennsylvania State University (U01 AG14263), the University of Alabama at Birmingham (U01 AG14289), and the University of Florida (U01 AG14276).

Footnotes

Financial Disclosures: Dr Ball owns stock in Visual Awareness Inc, which owns the patent for the Useful Field of View testing and training software. No other authors reported disclosures.

Role of the Sponsor: Representatives of the National Institute on Aging and the National Institute for Nursing Research were directly involved in the design of the study, interpretation of the data, and preparation, review, and approval of the manuscript. These representatives also monitored the conduct of the study, collection, management, and analysis of the data.

ACTIVE Study Investigators: Richard N. Jones, ScD, Adrienne L. Rosenberg, MS (Hebrew Senior life); Kathy Johnson, PhD, Daniel F. Rexroth, PsyD, David M. Smith, MD, Elizabeth Way, BA, Fredric D. Wolinsky, PhD (Indiana University School of Medicine); Kay Cresci, PhD, RN, Joseph Gallo, MD, MPH, Laura Talbot, PhD, EdD, RN, CS (Johns Hopkins University); Michael Doherty, MS, Patricia Forde, BS, Yan Xu, MS (New England Research Institutes, data coordinating center); Pamela Davis, MS, Scott Hofer, PhD, K. Warner Schaie, PhD (Pennsylvania State University); Jerri Edwards, PhD, Martha Frankel, Cynthia Owsley, PhD, Dan Roenker, PhD, David Vance, PhD, Virginia Wadley, PhD (University of Alabama at Birmingham); Manfred K. Diehl, PhD, Ann L. Horgas, RN, PhD, FAAN, Peter A lichtenberg, PhD, ABPP (University of Florida/Wayne State University).

REFERENCES

1. Kelly-Hayes M, Jette AM, Wolf PA, D’Agostino RB, Odell PM. Functional limitations and disability among elders in the Framingham Study. Am J Public Health. 1992;82:841–845. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
2. Stuck AE, Walthert JM, Nikolaus T, Bula CJ, Hohmann C, Beck JC. Risk factors for functional status decline in community-living elderly people: a systematic literature review. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48:445–469. [PubMed]
3. Wolinsky FD, Callahan CM, Fitzgerald JF, Johnson RJ. Changes in functional status and the risks of subsequent nursing home placement and death. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1993;48:S94–S101. [PubMed]
4. Willis S. Societal Mechanisms for Maintaining Competence in Old Age. Springer Publishing; New York, NY: 1997.
5. Royall DR, Palmer R, Chiodo LK, Polk MJ. Normal rates of cognitive change in successful aging: the freedom house study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11:899–909. [PubMed]
6. Davis RN, Massman PJ, Doody RS. Cognitive intervention in Alzheimer disease: a randomized placebocontrolled study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2001;15:1–9. [PubMed]
7. Cherry KE, Simmons-D’Gerolamo SS. Long-term effectiveness of spaced-retrieval memory training for older adults with probable Alzheimer’s disease. Exp Aging Res. 2005;31:261–289. [PubMed]
8. Rebok GW, Balcerak U. Memory self-efficacy and performance differences in young and old adults: effects of mnemonic training. Dev Psychol. 1989;25:714–721.
9. Willis SL. Cognitive training and everyday competence. Annu Rev Gerontol Geriatr. 1987;7:159–188. [PubMed]
10. O’Hara R, Brooks JO, III, Friedman L, Schroder CM, Morgan KS, Kraemer HC. Long-term effects of mnemonic training in community-dwelling older adults [published online ahead of print June 14, 2006] J Psychiatr Res. doi:1 0.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.04.01 O.
11. Derwinger A, Stigsdotter Neely A, MacDonald S, Backman L. Forgetting numbers in old age: strategy and learning speed matter. Gerontology. 2005;51:277–284. [PubMed]
12. Jobe JB, Smith DM, Ball K, et al. ACTIVE: a cognitive intervention trial to promote independence in older adults. Control Clin Trials. 2001;22:453–479. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
13. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288:2271–2281. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
14. Lazaridis EN, Rudberg MA, Furner SE, Cassel CK. Do activities of daily living have a hierarchical structure? an analysis using the longitudinal study of aging. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1994;49:M47–M51. [PubMed]
15. Wolinsky F, Miller D. Disability concepts and measurement: contributions of the epidemiology of disability to gerontological inquiry. In: Wilmoth J, Ferraro K, editors. Gerontology: Perspectives and Issues. 3rd ed Springer Publishing; New York, NY: 2006.
16. Roenker DL, Cissell GM, Ball KK, Wadley VG, Edwards JD. Speed-of-processing and driving simulator training result in improved driving performance. Hum Factors. 2003;45:218–233. [PubMed]
17. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. [PubMed]
18. Clark F, Azen SP, Zemke R, et al. Occupational therapy for independent-living older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 1997;278:1321–1326. [PubMed]
19. Willis SL, Cornelius SW, Blow FC, Baltes PB. Training research in aging: attentional processes. J Educ Psychol. 1983;75:257–270.
20. Rasmusson D, Rebok G, Bylsma F, Brandt J. Effects of three types of memory training in normal elderly. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 1999;6:56–66.
21. Willis SL, Schaie KW. Training the elderly on the ability factors of spatial orientation and inductive reasoning. Psychol Aging. 1986;1:239–247. [PubMed]
22. Ball K. Increased mobility and reducing accidents of older drivers. In: Schaie K, Pietrucha M, editors. Mobility and Transportation in the Elderly. Vol 5. Springer; New York, NY: 2000. pp. 213–250.
23. Edwards JD, Wadley VG, Myers RS, Roenker DL, Cissell GM, Ball KK. Transfer of a speed of processing intervention to near and far cognitive functions. Gerontology. 2002;48:329–340. [PubMed]
24. Brandt J. The Hopkins Verbal Learning Test: development of a new memory test with six equivalent forms. Clin Neuropsychol. 1991;5:125–142.
25. Rey A. L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encephalopathie tramatique. Arch Psychol. 1941;28:286–340.
26. Wilson B, Cockburn J, Baddeley A. The Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test. Thames Valley Test Co and National Rehabilitation Services; Reading, England and Gaylord, Mich: 1985.
27. Gonda J, Schaie K. Schaie-Thurstone Mental Abilities Test: Word Series Test. Consulting Psychologists Press; Palo Alto, Calif: 1985.
28. Thurstone L, Thurstone T. Examiner Manual for the SRA Primary Mental Abilities Test (Form 10-14) Science Research Associates; Chicago, III: 1949.
29. Ekstrom R, French J, Harman H, Derman D. Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests. Revised ed Educational Testing Service; Princeton, NJ: 1976.
30. Owsley C, Ball K, Sloane ME, Roenker DL, Bruni JR. Visual/cognitive correlates of vehicle accidents in older drivers. Psychol Aging. 1991;6:403–415. [PubMed]
31. Owsley C, Ball K, McGwin G, Jr, et al. Visual processing impairment and risk of motor vehicle crash among older adults. JAMA. 1998;279:1083–1088. [PubMed]
32. Ball KK, Beard BL, Roenker DL, Miller RL, Griggs DS. Age and visual search: expanding the useful field of view. J Opt Soc Am A. 1988;5:2210–2219. [PubMed]
33. Teresi J, Lawton M, Holmes D, Ory M. Measurement in elderly chronic care populations. In: Morris J, Morris S, editors. ADL Assessment Measures for Use With Frail Elders. Springer Publishing Co; New York, NY: 1997.
34. Willis S, Marsiske M. Pennsylvania State University; University Park: 1993. Manual for the Everyday Problems Test.
35. Diehl M, Marsiske M, Horgas AL, Rosenberg A, Saczynski J, Willis SL. The Revised Observed Tasks of Daily Living: a performance-based assessment of everyday problem solving in older adults. J Appl Gerontol. 2005;24:211–230. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
36. Owsley C, Sloane M, McGwin G, Jr, Ball K. Timed instrumental activities of daily living tasks: relationship to cognitive function and everyday performance assessments in older adults. Gerontology. 2002;48:254–265. [PubMed]
37. Cnaan A, Laird NM, Slasor P. Using the general linear mixed model to analyse unbalanced repeated measures and longitudinal data. Stat Med. 1997;16:2349–2380. [PubMed]
38. Blom G. Statistical Estimates and Transformed Beta-Variables. Wiley; New York, NY: 1958.
39. Lehmann E. Nonparametrics: Statistical Methods Based on Ranks. Holden-Day; San Francisco, Calif: 1975.
40. Schafer J. Analysis of Incomplete Multivariate Data. Chapman & Hall; London, England: 1997.
41. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc; Mahwah, NJ: 1988.
42. Schupf N, Tang MX, Albert SM, et al. Decline in cognitive and functional skills increases mortality risk in nondemented elderly. Neurology. 2005;65:1218–1226. [PubMed]
43. Allaire JC, Marsiske M. Well- and ill-defined measures of everyday cognition: relationship to older adults’ intellectual ability and functional status. Psychol Aging. 2002;17:101–115. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
44. Willis SL. Everyday cognitive competence in elderly persons: conceptual issues and empirical findings. Gerontologist. 1996;36:595–601. [PubMed]
45. Wadley VG, Harrell LE, Marson DC. Self- and informant report of financial abilities in patients with Alzheimer’s disease: reliable and valid? J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:1621–1626. [PubMed]
46. Koltai DC, Welsh-Bohmer KA, Schmechel DE. Influence of anosognosia on treatment outcome among dementia patients. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2001;11:455–475.
47. Allaire JC, Marsiske M. Everyday cognition: age and intellectual ability correlates. Psychol Aging. 1999;14:627–644. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
48. Wolinsky FD, Callahan CM, Fitzgerald JF, Johnson RJ. The risk of nursing home placement and subsequent death among older adults. J Gerontol. 1992;47:5173–5182. [PubMed]
49. Boyle PA, Paul RH, Moser DJ, Cohen RA. Executive impairments predict functional declines in vascular dementia. Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;18:75–82. [PubMed]
50. Evans DA, Bennett DA, Wilson RS, et al. Incidence of Alzheimer disease in a biracial urban community: relation to apolipoprotein E allele status. Arch Neurol. 2003;60:185–189. [PubMed]
51. Hendrie HC, Ogunniyi A, Hall KS, et al. Incidence of dementia and Alzheimer disease in 2 communities: Yoruba residing in Ibadan, Nigeria, and African Americans residing in Indianapolis, Indiana. JAMA. 2001;285:739–747. [PubMed]
52. Tang MX, Cross P, Andrews H, et al. Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and Caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology. 2001;56:49–56. [PubMed]
53. Fried TR, Bradley EH, Williams CS, Tinetti ME. Functional disability and health care expenditures for older persons. Arch Intern Med. 2001;161:2602–2607. [PubMed]
54. Plehn K, Marcopulos BA, McLain CA. The relationship between neuropsychological test performance, social functioning, and instrumental activities of daily living in a sample of rural older adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;18:101–113. [PubMed]
55. Kane R, Kane R, Ladd R. The Heart of Long-Term Care. Oxford University Press; New York, NY: 1998.
PubReader format: click here to try

Formats:

Related citations in PubMed

See reviews...See all...

Cited by other articles in PMC

See all...

Links

Recent Activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...