• We are sorry, but NCBI web applications do not support your browser and may not function properly. More information
Eur J Neurosci. Aug 2009; 30(3): 347–354.
PMCID: PMC2777251

Attention – oscillations and neuropharmacology

Abstract

Attention is a rich psychological and neurobiological construct that influences almost all aspects of cognitive behaviour. It enables enhanced processing of behaviourally relevant stimuli at the expense of irrelevant stimuli. At the cellular level, rhythmic synchronization at local and long-range spatial scales complements the attention-induced firing rate changes of neurons. The former is hypothesized to enable efficient communication between neuronal ensembles tuned to spatial and featural aspects of the attended stimulus. Recent modelling studies suggest that the rhythmic synchronization in the gamma range may be mediated by a fine balance between N-methyl-d-aspartate and α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methylisoxazole-4-propionate postsynaptic currents, whereas other studies have highlighted the possible contribution of the neuromodulator acetylcholine. This review summarizes some recent modelling and experimental studies investigating mechanisms of attention in sensory areas and discusses possibilities of how glutamatergic and cholinergic systems could contribute to increased processing abilities at the cellular and network level during states of top-down attention.

Keywords: acetylcholine, macaque monkey, modelling, neuronal coherence, N-methyl-d-aspartate, spatial integration

Introduction

Attention is necessary for all cognitive tasks, tasks that in their most simple form require stimulus selection, response selection and performance monitoring. It serves to tune out irrelevant information, enhance perceptual abilities and increase our readiness to respond appropriately. This is achieved by increased sensory representation of the attended objects at the neuronal level, which has been demonstrated for most visual cortical areas by means of single-unit recordings in macaque monkeys (Moran & Desimone, 1985; Spitzer et al., 1988; Motter, 1993; Treue & Maunsell, 1996; Roelfsema et al., 1998; Reynolds et al., 1999; Roberts et al., 2007) and by means of increased blood oxygenation level-dependent signals using functional magnetic resonance imaging in humans (Kastner et al., 1998; Tootell et al., 1998; Gandhi et al., 1999; Kastner & Ungerleider, 2000; Silver et al., 2005, 2007).

To understand how attention is implemented in the brain it is important to distinguish the control signals responsible for the generation and maintenance of attention (which originate in higher ‘source’ areas) from their effect on sensory processing and perception (which are visible in sensory areas as described in some detail below). Studies in humans and macaque monkeys have demonstrated that the control signals are mediated and maintained within circumscribed networks of the parietal and frontal cortex (e.g. Steinmetz & Constantinidis, 1995; Duncan et al., 1997; Thompson & Schall, 1999; Kusunoki et al., 2000; Everling et al., 2002, 2006; Shulman et al., 2002; Bisley & Goldberg, 2003; Mayer et al., 2004; Constantinidis & Steinmetz, 2005; Fan et al., 2005; Kincade et al., 2005). These areas control attentional selection and influence sensory processing and perception via feedback projections (Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Luck et al., 1997), as demonstrated by microstimulation studies (Moore & Armstrong, 2003; Armstrong & Moore, 2007; Moore & Fallah, 2004; Armstrong et al., 2006; Ekstrom et al., 2008) and by simultaneous recordings in sensory and source areas (Gregoriou et al., 2009). Although there are many open questions and possible scenarios regarding the implementation of feedback projections, here we will treat their existence as a ‘given’. Our focus will be on how attention affects neuronal network constellations in sensory areas to selectively alter activity level and pattern, possible mechanisms of its implementation and the contribution of selected neurotransmitters.

Attention and biased competition

Although a multitude of studies have demonstrated the effects of attention on basic stimulus processing, an experimental protocol used by Reynolds et al. (1999) allowed the explicit separation of sensory processing mechanisms from attentional effects. They demonstrated that the response of a V4 neuron to two stimuli in its receptive field in the absence of any attentional influences is not the sum of its responses to each stimulus alone. If one of the stimuli was an effective stimulus, whereas the other was an ineffective stimulus, the response to both stimuli presented simultaneously was a weighted average of the responses to each stimulus alone. Importantly, if spatial attention was allocated to the effective stimulus the neuronal activity was up-regulated towards the level of response corresponding to the effective stimulus alone. However, if spatial attention was allocated to the ineffective stimulus, then the neuronal activity was down-regulated towards the level of response elicited by the ineffective stimulus alone. Thus, attentional selection appears to operate by biasing an existing competitive interaction between multiple stimuli in the visual field toward one stimulus or another, so that behaviourally relevant stimuli are processed in the cortex, whereas irrelevant stimuli are filtered out (Duncan & Humphreys, 1989; Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Duncan & Nimmo-Smith, 1996).

Modelling biased competition

The synaptic and spiking mechanisms that may underlie biased competition have been analysed in a variety of different network models. Deco & Rolls (2005) have implemented a model of cortical areas V2 and V4 that exhibits biased competition signatures. Within this model both areas have the same internal architecture, with dynamic competition between neurons of different feature selectivity. Neurons were modelled as integrate and fire neurons with realistic synaptic N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA), α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methylisoxazole-4-propionate (AMPA) and γ-aminobutyric acid receptor dynamics (see Deco & Rolls, 2005 for details). Provided that the model is implemented with a particular connectivity, it generates dynamics that are consistent with the experimental results of biased competition (Reynolds et al., 1999), i.e. the dynamics show the level of competition and attentional biased up- and down-regulation observed experimentally in V2 and V4. Biased competition can be modelled in a variety of slightly different network configurations. Ardid et al. (2007) generated a reciprocally connected loop of a sensory and a working memory network comprised of biophysically-based spiking excitatory and inhibitory neurons. In this model the working memory module exhibited strong recurrent excitation, whereas the sensory network was dominated by inhibition. This model was able to replicate multiplicative gain changes, biased competition and feature similarity gain control, all of which have been reported to occur as a function of attention. Yet others have used modified versions of predictive coding to model biased competition in visual attention (Hamker, 2005; Spratling, 2008). A common feature of all of these models is that through dynamic competition a slight additional external input in one of the neuronal populations (bottom-up or top-down mediated) is amplified, whereas the activities of the other populations are partially suppressed, implementing in this way an effective filtering mechanism.

The role of neuronal synchrony: gamma band modulation

Oscillations in the gamma frequency band (30–100 Hz) have been found in most species and brain areas investigated, including the visual cortex (Gray et al., 1989; de Oliveira et al., 1997). Synchronization of neuronal activity in the gamma frequency band has been shown to be involved in several fundamental functions in the brain. Notably, neurons selected by attentional mechanisms show enhanced synchronization (Steinmetz et al., 2000), which is particularly prominent in the gamma band (Fries et al., 2001). Fries et al. (2001) recorded activity from area V4 while macaque monkeys were attending behaviourally relevant stimuli. They found that neurons activated by the attended stimulus showed increased gamma frequency synchronization compared with neurons activated by the distractor. A variety of different models have explored how synchrony in a network can be modulated in a manner similar to what is seen in V4 in attention-demanding tasks. Changes in synchrony occur in competing cortical interneuron networks (Tiesinga & Sejnowski, 2004) and attention-mediated increases in neuronal synchrony in the gamma range can be modelled by reducing adaptation currents in principal cells possibly through cholinergic mechanisms (Buhl et al., 1998; Borgers et al., 2005). Alternatively it can be mediated by decreasing the activity in inhibitory interneurons (Buia & Tiesinga, 2006), thereby releasing pyramidal cells from a ‘bath of inhibition’ (Borgers et al., 2008). The reduction of inhibitory drive on pyramidal cells might be mediated by cholinergic activation of interneuron–interneuron inhibition (Borgers et al., 2008). However, a cascade of interneuron interactions may not be necessary, as acetylcholine (ACh) can directly inactivate specific interneurons (Xiang et al., 1998).

All of the above models provide possible solutions as to how attention-mediated changes in neuronal synchrony might be implemented at the neuronal level but they do not address the question of whether synchrony plays a fundamental role in cognition. An attempt to provide some insight into this controversial topic was made by Buehlmann & Deco (2008) who investigated whether neuronal correlates of attention are rate-based (as demonstrated in biased competition) or oscillation-based (as demonstrated in the studies of Fries et al., 2001). They used the same network model with which biased competition had been studied (Deco & Rolls, 2005) but now delineated the parameters that allow for increased oscillation in the gamma range between neurons representing the attended location. Oscillations can be generated by a pyramidal-to-interneuron loop (Whittington et al., 1995; Traub et al., 1996; Brunel & Wang, 2003; Whittington & Traub, 2003; Gieselmann & Thiele, 2008), whereby the oscillation frequency depends on the relative timescales of the excitatory and inhibitory decay constants. Faster excitation than inhibition, or a higher excitation:inhibition ratio, favours the feedback loop and gives rise to oscillations in the gamma range (Brunel & Wang, 2003). In the network of Buehlmann & Deco (2008), oscillations were manipulated by adjusting the conductances of AMPA and NMDA receptors (gAMPA and gNMDA). An increase of gAMPA and a decrease of gNMDA allowed the pyramidal-to-interneuron loop interactions to oscillate more strongly and resulted in increased gamma activity. Within this network it was possible to adjust the conductances of AMPA and NMDA receptors (and thus the excitation/inhibition recurrent cycle) such that the network only showed gamma oscillations during stimulus presentation. Interestingly, these changes in the AMPA and NMDA receptor conductance of the network also determined whether attentional bias affected the rates more strongly or had a larger effect on the gamma synchronization (Buehlmann & Deco, 2008). This result is plotted in Fig. 1. It showed that rate modulation and gamma modulation in the network were not concomitant phenomena. Attentional rate modulation occurred for a broad range of the gAMPA:gNMDA ratios, whereas attentional modulation of the gamma band activity was restricted to a relatively narrow range of gAMPA:gNMDA ratios.

Fig. 1
Attention-induced rate modulation (solid line) and gamma modulation (dashed line) as a function of the gAMPA:gNMDA ratio in a network model. The initial main effect of increasing the gAMPA:gNMDA ratio (with or without attention) was an increase in the ...

Behavioural consequences of increased gamma synchronization

Attention-induced increases of gamma band modulation thus occur in vivo and in silico. The crucial question remains as to whether such changes have behavioural relevance. Additional analyses of the original data set of Fries et al. (2001) revealed that stronger gamma band modulations in V4 correlated with faster reaction times (RTs) of the monkeys (Womelsdorf et al., 2006). Equivalent results could be found in the network model of Buehlmann & Deco (2008). Their proxy of RT was firing rate-based (i.e. a neuronal RT). They analysed the time that it takes the stimulated neuron pool to rise to its mean firing frequency. Overall they found that neuronal RTs were different for the attended and unattended neuron pool, with attended pools exhibiting faster RTs. Importantly, they also found that it was necessary to keep the gAMPA:gNMDA conductance ratio within a fairly narrow range to produce the following four key results. (i) This narrow range of gAMPA:gNMDA conductance ratios generated the overall fastest neuronal RTs. (ii) It generated the largest RT differences between the attended and unattended pool. (iii) It allowed attention to strongly increase gamma modulations. (iv) Neuronal RTs inversely correlated with the attentional modulation in the gamma band, i.e. stronger attentional gamma band modulation led to shorter neuronal RTs. Thus, in-vivo (Womelsdorf et al., 2006) and in-silico (Buehlmann & Deco, 2008) results suggest a behavioural role for gamma synchronization during attentional selection.

The role of acetylcholine

In addition to feedback, the neuromodulator ACh, originating in the basal forebrain (Marrocco et al., 1994; Voytko et al., 1994; Voytko, 1996; Robbins, 1997; Witte et al., 1997; Sarter et al., 1999, 2001; Gill et al., 2000; Dalley et al., 2001), is likely to contribute to attention. Although cholinergic modulation of cortical processes can be signal driven, it can equally be top-down driven (Sarter et al., 2005; Parikh et al., 2007). Top-down-driven modulation of the corticopetal cholinergic system is under the control of ‘executive’ areas in the prefrontal cortex, thereby influencing their own ACh levels, those in parietal (Nelson et al., 2005) and sensory cortical areas. This form of top-down control enhances the cognitive modulation of detection processes (Nelson et al., 2005). Within this framework, the ascending cholinergic system can be conceptualized as part of the top-down control system emanating within the prefrontal cortex by which control is exerted over processing resources in the parietal attention system and in sensory cortices (Sarter et al., 2005). The cholinergic input to the cortex consists of modules that specifically target selected cortical areas in a task- and context-dependent manner (Zaborszky & Duque, 2000; Zaborszky, 2002). Complementary studies show that ACh release in the frontal cortex closely follows the time-course of attention-demanding events (Parikh et al., 2007) and behavioural studies in humans suggest that ACh (by means of muscarinic receptors) is responsible for the maintenance of attention but not for attention switching (Furey et al., 2008). Notably, increased levels of ACh are related to attentional effort, rather than attentional performance (Kozak et al., 2006).

Despite this wealth of data, it is unclear how ACh contributes to neuronal processing during selective attention. What are the cellular mechanisms by which ACh enables and boosts different aspect of attentional processing? Does ACh affect firing rates, does it affect synchrony, or both? Despite a lack of direct evidence for some of these questions, predictions can be based on slice studies, data from anaesthetized animals, as well as recent data in task-performing macaques. Early reports from cat V1 suggested that ACh increases the signal-to-noise ratio (Sillito & Kemp, 1983; Sato et al., 1987), which could be a mechanism by which attention acts. However, recordings in primate V1 failed to replicate this (Zinke et al., 2006). ACh reduces spike frequency adaptation, thus possibly mediating increased stability of stimulus or abstract representations at the single neuron level (McCormick & Prince, 1985, 1986), which would benefit attentional processing. It is tempting to speculate that ACh increases the representation of stimuli at the current attentional focus, and protects it against interference from competing stimuli, thus resulting in reduced distractibility. Thereby task-relevant information would be processed more effectively, which comes at the expense of irrelevant information. Recent data from our laboratory in the anaesthetized and awake primate are supportive of this idea. Based on the in-vitro studies we argued that ACh might alter the flow of feedforward and lateral/feedback information in the cortex. ACh suppresses the efficacy of intracortical synapses by activating muscarinic receptors (Hasselmo & Bower, 1992; Hasselmo, 1995; Kimura & Baughman, 1997; Kimura et al., 1999; Hsieh et al., 2000; Linster & Hasselmo, 2001) and increases the efficacy of feedforward/thalamocortical input (Gil et al., 1997) by acting on presynaptic nicotinic receptors located on thalamocortical synapses (Prusky et al., 1987). In the primary visual cortex, thalamocortical inputs are largely responsible for classical receptive field response properties (Dragoi et al., 2000), whereas non-classical receptive field influences are mostly mediated by lateral and feedback connections (Das & Gilbert, 1999; Angelucci et al., 2002; Cavanaugh et al., 2002). Thus, ACh applied in vivo should reduce the impact of stimuli presented in the non-classical receptive field while increasing the effect of stimuli placed within the classical receptive field. This proposal was investigated in V1 of anaesthetized marmoset monkeys (Roberts et al., 2005) by measuring the neuronal spatial integration properties. Roberts et al. (2005) predicted that spatial summation should be reduced when ACh was externally applied compared with when it was not applied. In line with this prediction, Roberts et al. (2005) found that the application of ACh resulted in a reduction of spatial integration, which was mediated by a reduction of the spatial summation area.

If ACh contributes to attentional modulation, then attention should reduce spatial integration at the perceptual and neuronal level. A variety of human psychophysical studies have indeed demonstrated that selective spatial attention can ensure that task-irrelevant information (information surrounding a target) has a reduced influence on perception. In these studies attention served to reduce the influence of behaviourally irrelevant contextual stimuli (Ito et al., 1998; Zenger et al., 2000; Roberts & Thiele, 2008) or served as a noise-exclusion mechanism when stimuli had to be detected in noisy environments (Lu & Dosher, 1998; Dosher & Lu, 2000; Lu et al., 2002). In line with the above prediction, Roberts et al. (2007) reported that attention reduced the spatial integration of parafoveal cells in V1 and the underlying changes were mediated by a change in the spatial summation area (Roberts et al., 2007). Similar results have recently been reported for V4. Attention strongly affects centre-surround interactions of V4 neurons (Sundberg et al., 2009), ensuring that distractive influences by irrelevant stimuli are filtered out.

A direct test of the proposal that ACh is part of the machinery of attentional modulation in visual cortex has been performed recently (Herrero et al., 2008). These authors combined iontophoretic pharmacological analysis of cholinergic receptors with single-cell recordings in V1 while rhesus macaque monkeys performed a task that demanded top-down spatial attention. Herrero et al. (2008) demonstrated that local ACh application significantly increased attentional modulation in V1 neurons (Fig. 2) and affected the animal’s behaviour. It was as if the animals had difficulties in disengaging attention from the location represented by the neurons that were influenced by ACh application. The effects of ACh on attentional modulation in V1 were mediated by muscarinic receptors. Interestingly, the nicotinic antagonist mecamylamine had no systematic effect on attentional modulation, whereas it reduced the overall neuronal gain. The latter was somewhat expected as nicotinic receptors in V1 are largely located on presynaptic thalamocortical terminals (Wonnacott, 1997) where nicotine increases the synaptic transmission efficacy (Disney et al., 2007) but at the same time the thalamocortical synapse was unlikely to be the main site of attentional modulation in V1.

Fig. 2
Influence of ACh on attentional modulation in V1. (A) Example of a single cell where the application of ACh resulted in enhanced attentional modulation. In the absence of ACh application the attentional modulation was small but significant (P < ...

Outlook

The data reviewed so far demonstrate that attention affects firing rates and neuronal synchrony in different cortical areas. Modelling work suggests that these two effects are complementary mechanisms. Attention-induced increases of gamma synchrony seem to depend on a finely tuned balance of NMDA/AMPA receptor activation. Attention-induced firing rate changes in V1 depend on cholinergic activation of muscarinic receptors. Intriguingly, increased cortical synchrony in the gamma band range can also be induced by stimulation of the mesencephalic reticular formation (Munk et al., 1996), which triggers increased levels of ACh in the cortex (Szerb, 1967). Moreover, stimulation of the nucleus basalis, the main source of cortical ACh, results in EEG activation in the auditory cortex and it changes cellular membrane potential fluctuations from high-amplitude low-frequency to small-amplitude gamma frequency oscillations (Metherate et al., 1992). Application of the muscarinic antagonist scopolamine in V1 of anaesthetized cats resulted in reductions of gamma oscillations but local application of ACh surprisingly failed to increase gamma power directly (Rodriguez et al., 2004), although it affected gamma synchronization at longer timescales. It is thus tempting to speculate that ACh contributes to increased gamma oscillations as seen during selective attention in V4 (Fries et al., 2001) in addition to the proposed balanced NMDA:AMPA activation ratio (Buehlmann & Deco, 2008). Future work on the mechanisms of attention might focus on delineating whether ACh contributes to changes in cortical synchrony during states of attention in addition to its contribution to firing rate changes (Herrero et al., 2008). In addition, it will be important to investigate whether a change of the ratio of gAMPA:gNMDA (e.g. by application of the NMDA antagonist 5-phosphono-dl-norvaline dl-2-amino-5-phosphonovaleric acid) will abolish attention-induced changes in gamma power as predicted by Buehlmann & Deco (2008). Notably, this modelling predicts that such manipulations should mostly affect the strengths of attention-induced gamma oscillations, whereas attention-induced rate modulations should be less strongly affected.

The possible contribution of NMDA receptors to the attention-mediated increase in gamma oscillation is intriguing, as cognitive disorders are often associated with abnormal neural synchronization (Uhlhaas & Singer, 2006) and in schizophrenia this seems partially related to NMDA receptor hypofunction (Lisman et al., 2008). Despite this apparent link, the NMDA receptor hypofunction and its effect on gamma synchronization in schizophrenia are hypothesized to reduce excitation of fast-spiking interneurons (Lisman et al., 2008), whereas NMDA receptor-mediated effects in the model were pyramidal cell based (Buehlmann & Deco, 2008). Should the model thus be modified to investigate how alternative NMDA receptor locations affect function? This may be the case but it is important to highlight that NMDA receptor contributions to gamma synchronization can vary greatly between areas (Roopun et al., 2008). Thus, caution is necessary when attempting to apply findings from one specific structure to another structure. In this respect it may be useful to highlight the differences between areas in terms of neuromodulator susceptibility that rides on top of the well-established similarity of cortical structure across areas. The similarity of cortical structure between areas has led to the useful concept of the canonical microcircuit (Douglas & Martin, 2004) but differences in receptor distribution from area to area (e.g. Disney et al., 2006; Disney & Aoki, 2008) could be viewed as a canonical microcircuit breaker, by which processing flexibility is achieved. Due to these differences in receptor distribution, location and density it will be important to obtain a detailed understanding of receptor distributions in different areas and their specific role in cognitive functions. For such an understanding, immunohistochemical, in-vitro, in-vivo and in-silico approaches need to be integrated.

There is additional evidence suggesting that NMDA receptors play a ‘special’ role in attention. A striking similarity exists between changes in neuronal gain that are induced by direct NMDA application and those induced by selective attention (Fox et al., 1990; Williford & Maunsell, 2006; Thiele et al., 2009). Recent work has suggested that feedback projections recruit synapses with NMDA:AMPA receptor ratios different from those of feedforward projections (Self et al., 2008). Although direct evidence for this scenario is lacking, it is interesting to note that attention affects mostly the sustained part of visual responses in V1 (e.g. Roelfsema et al., 1998) and a selective contribution of NMDA receptors to the sustained part of the response in the ventrobasal thalamus (Salt, 1986) has been demonstrated. In addition, slice studies have shown that cortico-cortical interactions between layer 4 cells are largely mediated by a variety of different NMDA receptors (Fleidervish et al., 1998). Finally, studies in the primary and secondary motor cortex of the macaque demonstrate that NMDA and non-NMDA receptors contribute to different aspects and epochs of motor preparation and execution (Shima & Tanji, 1993). This evidence shows that synapses in the brain show functional specialization in terms of NMDA and non-NMDA receptor density. At the same time, the proposal that NMDA receptor-rich synapses are selectively recruited during attention is not predicted by either slice studies or modelling work. AMPA and not NMDA receptors play a dominant role in the generation of gamma oscillations of hippocampal and cortical slices (Buhl et al., 1998; Fisahn et al., 1998; LeBeau et al., 2002). Modelling by Buehlmann & Deco (2008) predicts a specific ratio of AMPA:NMDA currents to be present in the ‘attend in’ and ‘attend away’ states, not necessarily attention-induced changes of this ratio. It will thus be interesting to study how neuronal gain is affected by attention when NMDA receptor antagonists are applied, as well as how this affects the ability of attention to selectively alter gamma local field potential power and spike field coherence.

It may also be interesting to determine the effects of NMDA receptor blockade on the frequency and overall strengths of gamma oscillations in the absence of attentional manipulations. In a pyramidal/interneuron network the frequency of oscillations is determined by the rise and decay time of the inhibitory and excitatory postsynaptic potentials (Brunel & Wang, 2003). Faster excitation than inhibition favours gamma frequency oscillations in the 30–80 Hz range and the ratio determines the frequency of oscillations (Brunel & Wang, 2003). Moreover, the ratio of pyramidal cell and interneuron contribution to the network drive affects the oscillation frequency (Brunel & Wang, 2003; Gieselmann & Thiele, 2008). Thus, in principle NMDA receptor activation should reduce the gamma oscillation frequency and gamma power. Conversely, NMDA receptor blockade should increase the gamma power and shift the dominant frequency towards higher values. If true, it might be worthwhile to investigate whether attention affects the frequency of gamma oscillation in the striate and extrastriate cortex, in addition to the overall power, as such effects might reveal some of the underlying receptor mechanisms.

The same theoretical and computational framework that has been used to study the contribution of NMDA and AMPA receptors can be extended for a detailed study of the modulatory effects of ACh in attention. Here it will be of interest to explore how ACh could mediate biased competition in a two-area network. It is likely that this will involve modifying the strength of the recurrent connections, strength of the feedforward (thalamocortical) input and level of Ca2+-dependent afterhyperpolarization adaptation. Parametric manipulation of these parameters in the model may reveal how each parameter contributes to attentional amplification and suppression in the biased competition network, and to what extent these changes are rate or synchrony based. If successful, the effect of cholinergic receptor agonist and antagonist on attention-mediated biased competition in V4 could be explored experimentally, in conjunction with possible NMDA receptor mechanism interactions.

Finally, it may be of interest to explore to what extent spatial attention and working memory are conceptually related (whereby top-down spatial attention is regarded as a spatial working memory signal), and to explore how these are related in terms of their dependence on cholinergic and NMDA mechanisms. Modelling suggests that persistent mnemonic activity can be mediated by synaptic reverberations in recurrent circuits enabled by NMDA receptors (Wang, 2001), whereas in-vitro studies showed that persistent memory-related activity in the entorhinal cortex depends on muscarinic receptor mechanisms (Egorov et al., 2002).

The questions outlined in this review, although challenging, can now be addressed directly in task-performing monkeys. Such experiments will provide specific insights into the mechanisms of attention. The approaches are ideally informed by detailed model predictions but ultimately these are empirical questions that have to be tested experimentally. It is unlikely that all open questions can be resolved by probing attentional modulations in a single cortical area, as different areas show differences regarding neuromodulator receptor distributions (Mesulam et al., 1983) and they also vary with respect to the receptor locations on specific cell types (Disney et al., 2006; Disney & Aoki, 2008). Ultimately it will be necessary to study how different neurotransmitters contribute to cognitive function in many (if not all) cortical and subcortical areas, where the specific roles of different receptors will have to be delineated for each area. We also need to understand how the different transmitter systems contribute to rate and/or synchrony coding schemes if we want to understand cognition at the mechanistic level. Studies over the last 20 years have paved the way to perform this analysis within the framework of attention and we are now slowly heading towards such an understanding.

Acknowledgments

A. Thiele was supported by the BBSRC (BBS/B/09325), the Wellcome Trust (070380/z/03/z), and the MRC (G0700976). G. Deco was supported by the European Project ‘Briansync’, by Spanish Research Project Grant BFU2007-61710, and by CONSOLIDER INGENIO 2010 (‘Bilingualism and Cognitive Neuroscience’).

Glossary

Abbreviations

ACh
acetylcholine
AMPA
α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methylisoxazole-4-propionate
gAMPA
conductance of α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methylisoxazole-4-propionate receptors
gNMDA
conductance of N-methyl-d-aspartate receptors
NMDA
N-methyl-d-aspartate
RT
reaction time

References

  • Angelucci A, Levitt JB, Lund JS. Anatomical origins of the classical receptive field and modulatory surround field of single neurons in macaque visual cortical area V1. Prog. Brain Res. 2002;136:373–388. [PubMed]
  • Ardid S, Wang XJ, Compte A. An integrated microcircuit model of attentional processing in the neocortex. J. Neurosci. 2007;27:8486–8495. [PubMed]
  • Armstrong KM, Moore T. Rapid enhancement of visual cortical response discriminability by microstimulation of the frontal eye field. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:9499–9504. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Armstrong KM, Fitzgerald JK, Moore T. Changes in visual receptive fields with microstimulation of frontal cortex. Neuron. 2006;50:791–798. [PubMed]
  • Bisley JW, Goldberg ME. Neuronal activity in the lateral intraparietal area and spatial attention. Science. 2003;299:81–86. [PubMed]
  • Borgers C, Epstein S, Kopell NJ. Background gamma rhythmicity and attention in cortical local circuits: a computational study. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2005;102:7002–7007. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Borgers C, Epstein S, Kopell NJ. Gamma oscillations mediate stimulus competition and attentional selection in a cortical network model. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:18023–18028. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Brunel N, Wang XJ. What determines the frequency of fast network oscillations with irregular neural discharges? I. Synaptic dynamics and excitation-inhibition balance. J. Neurophysiol. 2003;90:415–430. [PubMed]
  • Buehlmann A, Deco G. The neuronal basis of attention: rate versus synchronization modulation. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:7679–7686. [PubMed]
  • Buhl EH, Tamas G, Fisahn A. Cholinergic activation and tonic excitation induce persistent gamma oscillations in mouse somatosensory cortex in vitro. J. Physiol. 1998;513(Pt 1):117–126. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Buia C, Tiesinga P. Attentional modulation of firing rate and synchrony in a model cortical network. J. Comput. Neurosci. 2006;20:247–264. [PubMed]
  • Cavanaugh JR, Bair W, Movshon JA. Nature and interaction of signals from the receptive field center and surround in macaque V1 neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88:2530–2546. [PubMed]
  • Constantinidis C, Steinmetz MA. Posterior parietal cortex automatically encodes the location of salient stimuli. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:233–238. [PubMed]
  • Dalley JW, McGaughy J, O’Connell MT, Cardinal RN, Levita L, Robbins TW. Distinct changes in cortical acetylcholine and noradrenaline efflux during contingent and noncontingent performance of a visual attentional task. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:4908–4914. [PubMed]
  • Das A, Gilbert CD. Topography of contextual modulations mediated by short-range interactions in primary visual cortex. Nature. 1999;399:655–661. [PubMed]
  • Deco G, Rolls ET. Neurodynamics of biased competition and cooperation for attention: a model with spiking neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;94:295–313. [PubMed]
  • Desimone R, Duncan J. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 1995;18:193–222. [PubMed]
  • Disney AA, Aoki C. Muscarinic acetylcholine receptors in macaque V1 are most frequently expressed by parvalbumin-immunoreactive neurons. J. Comp. Neurol. 2008;507:1748–1762. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Disney AA, Domakonda KV, Aoki C. Differential expression of muscarinic acetylcholine receptors across excitatory and inhibitory cells in visual cortical areas V1 and V2 of the macaque monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 2006;499:49–63. [PubMed]
  • Disney AA, Aoki C, Hawken MJ. Gain modulation by nicotine in macaque v1. Neuron. 2007;56:701–713. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Dosher BA, Lu ZL. Noise exclusion in spatial attention. Psychol. Sci. 2000;11:139–146. [PubMed]
  • Douglas RJ, Martin KA. Neuronal circuits of the neocortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2004;27:419–451. [PubMed]
  • Dragoi V, Sharma J, Sur M. Adaptation-induced plasticity of orientation tuning in adult visual cortex. Neuron. 2000;28:287–298. [PubMed]
  • Duncan J, Humphreys GW. Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychol. Rev. 1989;96:433–458. [PubMed]
  • Duncan J, Nimmo-Smith I. Objects and attributes in divided attention: surface and boundary systems. Percept. Psychophys. 1996;58:1076–1084. [PubMed]
  • Duncan J, Humphreys G, Ward R. Competitive brain activity in visual attention. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1997;7:255–261. [PubMed]
  • Egorov AV, Hamam BN, Fransen E, Hasselmo ME, Alonso AA. Graded persistent activity in entorhinal cortex neurons. Nature. 2002;420:173–178. [PubMed]
  • Ekstrom LB, Roelfsema PR, Arsenault JT, Bonmassar G, Vanduffel W. Bottom-up dependent gating of frontal signals in early visual cortex. Science. 2008;321:414–417. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Everling S, Tinsley CJ, Gaffan D, Duncan J. Filtering of neural signals by focused attention in the monkey prefrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2002;5:671–676. [PubMed]
  • Everling S, Tinsley CJ, Gaffan D, Duncan J. Selective representation of task-relevant objects and locations in the monkey prefrontal cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006;23:2197–2214. [PubMed]
  • Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI. The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage. 2005;26:471–479. [PubMed]
  • Fisahn A, Pike FG, Buhl EH, Paulsen O. Cholinergic induction of network oscillations at 40 Hz in the hippocampus in vitro. Nature. 1998;394:186–189. [PubMed]
  • Fleidervish IA, Binshtok AM, Gutnick MJ. Functionally distinct NMDA receptors mediate horizontal connectivity within layer 4 of mouse barrel cortex. Neuron. 1998;21:1055–1065. [PubMed]
  • Fox K, Sato H, Daw N. The effect of varying stimulus intensity on NMDA-receptor activity in cat visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1990;64:1413–1428. [PubMed]
  • Fries P, Reynolds JH, Rorie AE, Desimone R. Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science. 2001;291:1560–1563. [PubMed]
  • Furey ML, Pietrini P, Haxby JV, Drevets WC. Selective effects of cholinergic modulation on task performance during selective attention. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:913–923. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Gandhi SP, Heeger DJ, Boynton GM. Spatial attention affects brain activity in human primary visual cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1999;96:3314–3319. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Gieselmann MA, Thiele A. Comparison of spatial integration and surround suppression characteristics in spiking activity and the local field potential in macaque V1. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:447–459. [PubMed]
  • Gil Z, Connors BW, Amitai Y. Differential regulation of neocortical synapses by neuromodulators and activity. Neuron. 1997;19:679–686. [PubMed]
  • Gill TM, Sarter M, Givens B. Sustained visual attention performance-associated prefrontal neuronal activity: evidence for cholinergic modulation. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:4745–4757. [PubMed]
  • Gray CM, König P, Engel AK, Singer W. Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects global stimulus properties. Nature. 1989;338:334–337. [PubMed]
  • Gregoriou GG, Gotts SJ, Zhou H, Desimone R. High-frequency, long-range coupling between prefrontal and visual cortex during attention. Science. 2009;324:1207–1210. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hamker FH. The reentry hypothesis: the putative interaction of the frontal eye field, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, and areas V4, IT for attention and eye movement. Cereb. Cortex. 2005;15:431–447. [PubMed]
  • Hasselmo ME. Neuromodulation and cortical function: modeling the physiological basis of behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 1995;67:1–27. [PubMed]
  • Hasselmo ME, Bower JM. Cholinergic suppression specific to intrinsic not afferent fiber synapses in rat piriform (olfactory) cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1992;67:1222–1229. [PubMed]
  • Herrero JL, Roberts MJ, Delicato LS, Gieselmann MA, Dayan P, Thiele A. Acetylcholine contributes through muscarinic receptors to attentional modulation in V1. Nature. 2008;454:1110–1114. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Hsieh CY, Cruikshank SJ, Metherate R. Differential modulation of auditory thalamocortical and intracortical synaptic transmission by cholinergic agonist. Brain Res. 2000;880:51–64. [PubMed]
  • Ito M, Westheimer G, Gilbert CD. Attention and perceptual learning modulate contextual influences on visual perception. Neuron. 1998;20:1191–1197. [PubMed]
  • Kastner S, Ungerleider LG. Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2000;23:315–341. [PubMed]
  • Kastner S, De Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG. Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by functional MRI. Science. 1998;282:108–111. [PubMed]
  • Kimura F, Baughman RW. Distinct muscarinic receptor subtypes suppress excitatory and inhibitory synaptic responses in cortical neurons. J. Neurophysiol. 1997;77:709–716. [PubMed]
  • Kimura F, Fukuda M, Tsumoto T. Acetylcholine suppresses the spread of excitation in the visual cortex revealed by optical recording: possible differential effect depending on the source of input. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1999;11:3597–3609. [PubMed]
  • Kincade JM, Abrams RA, Astafiev SV, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study of voluntary and stimulus-driven orienting of attention. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:4593–4604. [PubMed]
  • Kozak R, Bruno JP, Sarter M. Augmented prefrontal acetylcholine release during challenged attentional performance. Cereb. Cortex. 2006;16:9–17. [PubMed]
  • Kusunoki M, Gottlieb J, Goldberg ME. The lateral intraparietal area as a salience map: the representation of abrupt onset, stimulus motion, and task relevance. Vision Res. 2000;40:1459–1468. [PubMed]
  • LeBeau FE, Towers SK, Traub RD, Whittington MA, Buhl EH. Fast network oscillations induced by potassium transients in the rat hippocampus in vitro. J. Physiol. 2002;542:167–179. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Linster C, Hasselmo ME. Neuromodulation and the functional dynamics of piriform cortex. Chem. Senses. 2001;26:585–594. [PubMed]
  • Lisman JE, Coyle JT, Green RW, Javitt DC, Benes FM, Heckers S, Grace AA. Circuit-based framework for understanding neurotransmitter and risk gene interactions in schizophrenia. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31:234–242. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Lu ZL, Dosher BA. External noise distinguishes attention mechanisms. Vision Res. 1998;38:1183–1198. [PubMed]
  • Lu ZL, Lesmes LA, Dosher BA. Spatial attention excludes external noise at the target location. J. Vis. 2002;2:312–323. [PubMed]
  • Luck SJ, Chelazzi L, Hillyard SA, Desimone R. Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1997;77:24–42. [PubMed]
  • Marrocco RT, Witte EA, Davidson MC. Arousal systems. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1994;4:166–170. [PubMed]
  • Mayer AR, Dorflinger JM, Rao SM, Seidenberg M. Neural networks underlying endogenous and exogenous visual-spatial orienting. Neuroimage. 2004;23:534–541. [PubMed]
  • McCormick DA, Prince DA. Two types of muscarinic response to acetylcholine in mammalian cortical neurons. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1985;82:6344–6348. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • McCormick DA, Prince DA. Mechanisms of action of acetylcholine in the guinea-pig cerebral cortex in vitro. J. Physiol. 1986;375:169–194. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Mesulam M-M, Mufson EJ, Levey AI, Wainer BH. Cholinergic innervation of cortex by the basal forebrain: cytochemistry and cortical connections of the septal area, diagonal band nuclei, nucleus basalis (substantia innominata), and hypothalamus in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 1983;214:170–197. [PubMed]
  • Metherate R, Cox CL, Ashe JH. Cellular bases of neocortical activation: modulation of neural oscillations by the nucleus basalis and endogenous acetylcholine. J. Neurosci. 1992;12:4701–4711. [PubMed]
  • Moore T, Armstrong KM. Selective gating of visual signals by microstimulation of frontal cortex. Nature. 2003;421:370–373. [PubMed]
  • Moore T, Fallah M. Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention. J. Neurophysiol. 2004;91:152–162. [PubMed]
  • Moran J, Desimone R. Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. Science. 1985;229:782–784. [PubMed]
  • Motter BC. Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. J. Neurophysiol. 1993;70:909–919. [PubMed]
  • Munk MHJ, Roelfsma PR, König P, Engel AK, Singer W. Role of reticular activation in the modulation of intracortical synchronization. Science. 1996;272:271–274. [PubMed]
  • Nelson CL, Sarter M, Bruno JP. Prefrontal cortical modulation of acetylcholine release in posterior parietal cortex. Neuroscience. 2005;132:347–359. [PubMed]
  • de Oliveira SC, Thiele A, Hoffmann KP. Synchronization of neuronal activity during stimulus expectation in a direction discrimination task. J. Neurosci. 1997;17:9248–9260. [PubMed]
  • Parikh V, Kozak R, Martinez V, Sarter M. Prefrontal acetylcholine release controls cue detection on multiple timescales. Neuron. 2007;56:141–154. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Prusky GT, Shaw C, Cynader MS. Nicotine receptors are located on lateral geniculate nucleus terminals in cat visual cortex. Brain Res. 1987;412:131–138. [PubMed]
  • Reynolds JH, Chelazzi L, Desimone R. Competitive mechanisms subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:1736–1753. [PubMed]
  • Robbins TW. Arousal systems and attentional processes. Biol. Psychol. 1997;45:57–71. [PubMed]
  • Roberts MJ, Thiele A. Attention and contrast differently affect contextual integration in an orientation discrimination task. Exp. Brain Res. 2008;187:535–549. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Roberts MJ, Zinke W, Guo K, Robertson R, McDonald JS, Thiele A. Acetylcholine dynamically controls spatial integration in marmoset primary visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;93:2062–2072. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Roberts M, Delicato LS, Herrero J, Gieselmann MA, Thiele A. Attention alters spatial integration in macaque V1 in an eccentricity-dependent manner. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:1483–1491. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Rodriguez R, Kallenbach U, Singer W, Munk MH. Short- and long-term effects of cholinergic modulation on gamma oscillations and response synchronization in the visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2004;24:10369–10378. [PubMed]
  • Roelfsema PR, Lamme VA, Spekreijse H. Object-based attention in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Nature. 1998;395:376–381. [PubMed]
  • Roopun AK, Cunningham MO, Racca C, Alter K, Traub RD, Whittington MA. Region-specific changes in gamma and beta2 rhythms in NMDA receptor dysfunction models of schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2008;34:962–973. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Salt TE. Mediation of thalamic sensory input by both NMDA receptors and non-NMDA receptors. Nature. 1986;322:263–265. [PubMed]
  • Sarter M, Bruno JP, Turchi J. Basal forebrain afferent projections modulating cortical acetylcholine, attention, and implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1999;877:368–382. [PubMed]
  • Sarter M, Givens B, Bruno JP. The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 2001;35:146–160. [PubMed]
  • Sarter M, Hasselmo ME, Bruno JP, Givens B. Unraveling the attentional functions of cortical cholinergic inputs: interactions between signal-driven and cognitive modulation of signal detection. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 2005;48:98–111. [PubMed]
  • Sato H, Hata Y, Masui H, Tsumoto T. A functional role of cholinergic innervation to neurons in the cat visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 1987;58:765–780. [PubMed]
  • Self M, Super H, Roelfsema P. Feedforward and feedback visual processing utilise different glutamate receptors. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 2008;38:769.714.
  • Shima K, Tanji J. Involvement of NMDA and non-NMDA receptors in motor task-related activity in the primary and secondary cortical motor areas of the monkey. Cereb. Cortex. 1993;3:330–347. [PubMed]
  • Shulman GL, d’Avossa G, Tansy AP, Corbetta M. Two attentional processes in the parietal lobe. Cereb. Cortex. 2002;12:1124–1131. [PubMed]
  • Sillito AM, Kemp JA. Cholinergic modulation of the functional organization of the cat visual cortex. Brain Res. 1983;289:143–155. [PubMed]
  • Silver MA, Ress D, Heeger DJ. Topographic maps of visual spatial attention in human parietal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2005;94:1358–1371. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Silver MA, Ress D, Heeger DJ. Neural correlates of sustained spatial attention in human early visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 2007;97:229–237. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Spitzer H, Desimone R, Moran J. Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance. Science. 1988;240:338–340. [PubMed]
  • Spratling MW. Predictive coding as a model of biased competition in visual attention. Vision Res. 2008;48:1391–1408. [PubMed]
  • Steinmetz MA, Constantinidis C. Neurophysiological evidence for a role of posterior parietal cortex in redirecting visual attention. Cereb. Cortex. 1995;5:448–456. [PubMed]
  • Steinmetz PN, Roy A, Fitzgerald PJ, Hsiao SS, Johnston KO, Niebur E. Attention modulates synchronized neuronal firing in primate somatosensory cortex. Nature. 2000;404:187–190. [PubMed]
  • Sundberg KA, Mitchell JF, Reynolds JH. Spatial attention modulates center-surround interactions in macaque visual area v4. Neuron. 2009;61:952–963. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Szerb JC. Cortical acetylcholine release and electroencephalographic arousal. J. Physiol. 1967;192:329–343. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Thiele A, Pooresmaeili A, Delicato LS, Herrero JL, Roelfsema PR. Additive effects of attention and stimulus contrast in primary visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2009 epub ahead of print [doi 10.1093/cercor/bhp070. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Thompson KG, Schall JD. The detection of visual signals by macaque frontal eye field during masking. Nat. Neurosci. 1999;2:283–288. [PubMed]
  • Tiesinga PH, Sejnowski TJ. Rapid temporal modulation of synchrony by competition in cortical interneuron networks. Neural Comput. 2004;16:251–275. [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • Tootell RB, Hadjikhani N, Hall EK, Marrett S, Vanduffel W, Vaughan JT, Dale AM. The retinotopy of visual spatial attention. Neuron. 1998;21:1409–1422. [PubMed]
  • Traub RD, Whittington MA, Stanford IM, Jefferys JGR. A mechanism for generation of long-range synchronous fast oscillations in the cortex. Nature. 1996;383:621–624. [PubMed]
  • Treue S, Maunsell JHR. Attentional modulation of visual motion processing in cortical areas MT and MST. Nature. 1996;382:539–541. [PubMed]
  • Uhlhaas PJ, Singer W. Neural synchrony in brain disorders: relevance for cognitive dysfunctions and pathophysiology. Neuron. 2006;52:155–168. [PubMed]
  • Voytko ML. Cognitive functions of the basal forebrain cholinergic system in monkeys: memory or attention. Behav. Brain Res. 1996;75:13–25. [PubMed]
  • Voytko ML, Olton DS, Richardson RT, Gorman LK, Tobin JR, Price DL. Basal forebrain lesions in monkeys disrupt attention but not learning and memory. J. Neurosci. 1994;14:167–186. [PubMed]
  • Wang XJ. Synaptic reverberation underlying mnemonic persistent activity. Trends Neurosci. 2001;24:455–463. [PubMed]
  • Whittington MA, Traub RD. Interneuron diversity series: inhibitory interneurons and network oscillations in vitro. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:676–682. [PubMed]
  • Whittington MA, Traub RD, Jefferys JG. Synchronized oscillations in interneuron networks driven by metabotropic glutamate receptor activation. Nature. 1995;373:612–615. [PubMed]
  • Williford T, Maunsell JH. Effects of spatial attention on contrast response functions in macaque area V4. J. Neurophysiol. 2006;96:40–54. [PubMed]
  • Witte EA, Davidson MC, Marrocco RT. Effects of altering brain cholinergic activity on covert orienting of attention: comparison of monkey and human performance. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 1997;132:324–334. [PubMed]
  • Womelsdorf T, Fries P, Mitra PP, Desimone R. Gamma-band synchronization in visual cortex predicts speed of change detection. Nature. 2006;439:733–736. [PubMed]
  • Wonnacott S. Presynaptic nicotinic ACh receptors. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20:92–98. [PubMed]
  • Xiang Z, Huguenard JR, Prince DA. Cholinergic switching within neocortical inhibitory networks. Science. 1998;281:985–988. [PubMed]
  • Zaborszky L. The modular organization of brain systems. Basal forebrain: the last frontier. Prog. Brain Res. 2002;136:359–372. [PubMed]
  • Zaborszky L, Duque A. Local synaptic connections of basal forebrain neurons. Behav. Brain Res. 2000;115:143–158. [PubMed]
  • Zenger B, Braun J, Koch C. Attentional effects on contrast detection in the presence of surround masks. Vision Res. 2000;40:3717–3724. [PubMed]
  • Zinke W, Roberts MJ, Guo K, McDonald JS, Robertson R, Thiele A. Cholinergic modulation of response properties and orientation tuning of neurons in primary visual cortex of anaesthetized Marmoset monkeys. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2006;24:314–328. [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Articles from Wiley-Blackwell Online Open are provided here courtesy of Wiley-Blackwell, John Wiley & Sons
PubReader format: click here to try

Formats: