• We are sorry, but NCBI web applications do not support your browser and may not function properly. More information
Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptNIH Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
Dyslexia. Author manuscript; available in PMC Aug 1, 2010.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2739722
NIHMSID: NIHMS141978

Gender ratios for reading difficulties

Abstract

The prevalence of reading difficulties is typically higher in males than females in both referred and research-identified samples, and the ratio of males to females is greater in more affected samples. To explore possible gender differences in reading performance, we analyzed data from 1,133 twin pairs in which at least one member of each pair had a school-history of reading problems and from 684 twin pairs from a comparison sample with no reading difficulties. Although the difference between the average scores of males and females in these two samples was very small, the variance of reading performance was significantly greater for males in both groups. We suggest that a greater variance of reading performance measures in males may account at least in part for their higher prevalence of reading difficulties as well as for the higher gender ratios that are observed in more severely impaired samples.

Keywords: Dyslexia, Gender Ratio, Prevalence, Reading Disability, Variance

The ratio of males to females in samples of children with reading difficulties varies widely depending upon the method of ascertainment. In studies where subjects are ascertained employing clinical or referral methods, gender ratios range from 2:1 to 15:1 males to females (e.g., Finucci & Childs, 1981; Vogel, 1990); however, in research-identified samples, gender ratios are closer to 1:1 (e.g., Harlaar, Spinath, Dale, & Plomin, 2005; Hawke, Wadsworth, Olson, & DeFries, 2007; Shaywitz, Shaywitz, Fletcher, & Escobar, 1990; Stevenson, 1992). Nevertheless, in both referred and research-identified samples, greater numbers of males with reading problems have typically been reported. For example, in a recent review of sex differences in reading disability, Rutter et al. (2004) reported the gender ratios in four independent epidemiological studies in which the samples had been ascertained using research criteria. In all four of the studies, significantly more males than females with reading disabilities were reported. Moreover, gender ratios for reading difficulties are greater in more severely affected samples (Hawke et al., 2007; Olson, 2002).

Several biological and environmental hypotheses have been proposed to account for this gender difference in prevalence rates including X-linked recessive inheritance (Symmes & Rapoport, 1972), differences in brain functioning due to differential exposure or sensitivity to androgens (Geschwind, 1981; Nass, 1993; Tallal & Fitch, 1993), immunological factors, sexual imprinting, perinatal complications, and differential resilience to neural insult (Liederman, Kantrowitz, & Flannery, 2005). It has also been suggested that females may be less susceptible to environmental factors such as teaching methods and socioeconomic status (Geschwind, 1981), and that genetic influences may be more important as a cause of reading difficulties in females than in males (DeFries & Gillis, 1993; Stevenson, 1992). However, in this brief report, we suggest a more parsimonious explanation for the greater prevalence of reading difficulties in males and for higher gender ratios with increasing severity.

We recently analyzed reading performance data [a composite measure of Reading Recognition (REC), Reading Comprehension (COMP) and Spelling (SPELL) subtest scores from the Peabody Individual Achievement Test (Dunn & Markwardt, 1970)] from 1,133 twin pairs in which at least one member of each pair had a positive school history of reading difficulties (Group I) and from a comparison sample of 684 twin pairs with no reading difficulties (Group II). (See Hawke et al., 2007, for a more detailed description of the measure and method used for sample ascertainment.) As expected, the difference between the means of the two groups was large [F(1, 3624) = 1415.82, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.28]. In contrast, the difference between the mean scores of males and females was very small [F(1, 3624) = 0.671, p = 0.41, η2 = 0.0001]. However, the variance of males was larger than that of females in both Group I [F(1166, 1094) = 1.31, p < 0.001] and Group II [F(630, 731) = 1.18, p = 0.02]. Similar results were previously reported by Reynolds et al. (1996), who found that the phenotypic variance for oral reading performance was greater for males than for females in the Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behavioral Development, and that the mean scores of males and females were not significantly different. Other investigators (e.g., Hedges & Nowell, 1995; Machin & Pekkarinen, 2008) have also previously noted that mental test scores of males consistently have larger variances than those of females even though average gender differences are relatively small.

This increased variance in males can account for the paradoxical finding that the prevalence of reading difficulties is higher in males than in females and that this gender ratio increases in more severely impaired samples, even in the absence of a difference between their means. For example, the ratio of the standard deviation (SD) of reading performance in males to that in females in Group I is 1.15. Therefore, using diagnostic cutoffs corresponding to −1.0, −2.0 or −3.0 SDs in males would correspond to −1.15, −2.30 and −3.45 SD cutoffs in females. Consequently, using the −1.0 SD cutoff for males, 15.9% of the males would be selected and 12.5% of the females would be selected, resulting in a gender ratio of 1.27. However, using cutoffs that correspond to −2.0 or −3.0 SDs in males, the gender ratios would be 2.13 or 4.33, respectively. Figure 1 illustrates how a greater proportion of males than females will be included in selected samples, and how this gender ratio will increase as a function of severity, even when their population means are exactly equal.

Figure 1
Theoretical distributions of scores for males (red distribution, SD = 1.0) and females (blue distribution, SD = 0.85).

Thus, the higher prevalence of reading difficulties in males, especially in more highly selected samples, may be due to their greater variance for reading performance. Because our measure of reading performance is a composite measure of three subtest scores, variance differences for males and females on one or all of these measures, and/or gender differences in correlations among the three measures, could lead to gender differences in variance for reading performance. In fact, variances of males are significantly larger than those of females for REC, COMP, and SPELL in Group I and for REC and SPELL in Group II [Group I: REC, F(1169, 1094) = 1.28, p < 0.001; COMP, F(1166, 1094) = 1.20, p = 0.001; SPELL, F(1170, 1094) = 1.19, p = 0.002; Group II: REC, F(633, 731) = 1.15, p = 0.03; SPELL, F(634, 732) = 1.18, p = 0.02], and phenotypic correlations among the measures are consistently higher in males than in females in both groups. These results clearly suggest that the greater variance of our composite measure of reading performance for males is due to the gender differences in phenotypic variances and covariances of REC, COMP, and SPELL. Future research is warranted to assess why the variances of reading-related measures are higher in males than in females.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by center grant HD-27802 and training grant HD-07289 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. The invaluable contributions of the twins, their families and the staff members of the school districts who participated in this study are gratefully acknowledged.

Contributor Information

Jesse L. Hawke, University of Colorado, Boulder, Institute for Behavioral Genetics, 447 UCB, Boulder, CO 80309-0447, Telephone: (303) 735-6179, E-mail: ude.odaroloc@jekwah, Fax: (303) 492-8063.

Richard K. Olson, University of Colorado, Boulder.

Erik G. Willcutt, University of Colorado, Boulder.

Sally J. Wadsworth, University of Colorado, Boulder.

John C. DeFries, University of Colorado, Boulder.

References

  • DeFries JC, Gillis JJ. Genetics of reading disabilities. In: Plomin R, McClearn GE, editors. Nature, nurture and psychology. Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association; 1993. pp. 121–145.
  • Dunn ML, Markwardt FC. Examiner’s Manual: Peabody Individual Achievement Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1970.
  • Finucci JM, Childs B. Are there really more dyslexic boys than girls? In: Ansara A, Geschwind N, Galaburda A, Albert M, Gartrell N, editors. Sex differences in dyslexia. Townson, MD: Orton Dyslexia; 1981. pp. 1–9.
  • Geschwind N. A reaction to the conference on sex differences in dyslexia. In: Ansara A, Geschwind N, Galaburda A, Albert M, Gartrell N, editors. Sex differences in dyslexia. Townson, MD: Orton Dyslexia; 1981. pp. xiii–xviii.
  • Harlaar N, Spinath FM, Dale PS, Plomin R. Genetic influences on early word recognition abilities and disabilities: A study of 7-year-old twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2005;46:373–384. [PubMed]
  • Hawke JL, Wadsworth SJ, Olson RK, DeFries JC. Etiology of reading difficulties as a function of gender and severity. Reading and Writing. 2007;20:13–25.
  • Hedges LV, Nowell A. Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science. 1995;269:41–45. [PubMed]
  • Liederman J, Kantrowitz L, Flannery K. Male vulnerability to reading disability is not likely to be a myth: A call for new data. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 2005;38:109–129. [PubMed]
  • Machin S, Pekkarinen T. Global sex differences in test score variability. Science. 2008;322:1331–1332. [PubMed]
  • Nass R. Sex differences in learning abilities and disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia. 1993;43:61–77. [PubMed]
  • Olson RK. Dyslexia: Nature and nurture. Dyslexia. 2002;8:143–159. [PubMed]
  • Reynolds CA, Hewitt JK, Erickson MT, Silberg JL, Rutter M, Simonoff E, Meyer J, Eaves LJ. The genetics of children’s oral reading performance. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1996;37:425–434. [PubMed]
  • Rutter M, Caspi A, Fergusson D, Horwood LJ, Goodman R, Maughan B, Moffitt TE, Meltzer H, Carroll J. Sex differences in developmental reading disability: New findings from 4 epidemiological studies. JAMA. 2004;291:2007–2012. [PubMed]
  • Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Fletcher JM, Escobar MD. Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls. JAMA. 1990;291:998–1002. [PubMed]
  • Stevenson J. Identifying sex differences in reading disability: Lessons from a twin study. Reading and Writing. 1992;4:307–326.
  • Symmes JS, Rapoport JL. Unexpected reading failure. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 1972;42:82–91. [PubMed]
  • Tallal P, Fitch RH. Hormones and cerebral organization: Implications for the development and transmission of language and learning disabilities. In: Galaburda AM, editor. Dyslexia and development: Neurobiological aspects of extra-ordinary brains. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1993. pp. 168–186.
  • Vogel SA. Gender differences in intelligence, language, visual-motor abilities, and academic achievement in students with learning disabilities: A review of the literature. Journal of Learning Disabilities. 1990;23:44–52. [PubMed]
PubReader format: click here to try

Formats:

Related citations in PubMed

See reviews...See all...

Cited by other articles in PMC

See all...

Links

  • MedGen
    MedGen
    Related information in MedGen
  • PubMed
    PubMed
    PubMed citations for these articles

Recent Activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...